Published on in Vol 8, No 1 (2021): Jan-Mar

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/23232, first published .
Information Sources, Risk Perception, and Efficacy Appraisal’s Prediction of Engagement in Protective Behaviors Against COVID-19 in China: Repeated Cross-sectional Survey

Information Sources, Risk Perception, and Efficacy Appraisal’s Prediction of Engagement in Protective Behaviors Against COVID-19 in China: Repeated Cross-sectional Survey

Information Sources, Risk Perception, and Efficacy Appraisal’s Prediction of Engagement in Protective Behaviors Against COVID-19 in China: Repeated Cross-sectional Survey

Authors of this article:

Jian Raymond Rui1 Author Orcid Image ;   Keqing Yang1 Author Orcid Image ;   Juan Chen1 Author Orcid Image

Original Paper

College of Journalism and Communication, South China University of Technology, Guangzhou, China

*all authors contributed equally

Corresponding Author:

Keqing Yang, PhD

College of Journalism and Communication

South China University of Technology

382 Waihuan East Rd

Guangzhou, 510006

China

Phone: 86 17819720631

Email: jckqyang@scut.edu.cn


Background: As the COVID-19 pandemic has become a major public health threat worldwide, it is critical to understand what factors affect individual engagement in protective actions. Because of its authoritarian political system and state-owned media system, how Chinese individuals engaged in protective actions against COVID-19 might be different compared to other countries.

Objective: The purpose of this study is to examine how the source of information about COVID-19, Chinese individuals’ risk perception of COVID-19 (ie, perceived severity and perceived susceptibility), and their efficacy appraisal in controlling COVID-19 (ie, response efficacy and self-efficacy) affected their engagement in protective actions. Additionally, this study aims to investigate whether there is any difference in these relationships throughout the duration of this pandemic.

Methods: A six-wave repeated cross-sectional survey (N=1942) was conducted in six major cities in China between February 7 and April 23, 2020. Participants’ reliance on expert versus inexpert sources for information about COVID-19, their perceived severity of and susceptibility to COVID-19, their response efficacy and self-efficacy, and their engagement in protective actions (staying at home, wearing a face mask, and washing hands) were measured. Demographic variables (sex, age, income, education, and city of residence), knowledge of COVID-19, and self-rated health condition were controlled.

Results: Reliance on expert sources did not become the major factor that motivated these actions until wave 3, and the negative effect of inexpert sources on these actions was limited to wave 2. Perceived severity encouraged some protective behaviors but its effect varied depending on the specific behavior. In addition, perceived severity exhibited a stronger effect on these behaviors compared to perceived susceptibility. The positive effect of response efficacy was only significant at waves 1 and 2, and limited to certain behaviors.

Conclusions: Chinese individuals’ engagement in protective behaviors might not entirely be their autonomous decision but a result of compliance with executive orders. After the early outbreak, expert sources started to facilitate protective behaviors, suggesting that it might take time to develop trust in these sources. The facilitating effect of perceived severity lasted throughout the duration of the pandemic, but that of response efficacy was limited to the early stage.

JMIR Hum Factors 2021;8(1):e23232

doi:10.2196/23232

Keywords



Background

Having spread to 188 countries and regions [1], COVID-19 has become a serious public health threat worldwide. As of October 16, 2020, COVID-19 has caused over 38 million cases and nearly 1,100,000 deaths [1]. China is the first country where COVID-19 was discovered. As early as December 2019, COVID-19 was found in Wuhan, China [2]. The number of confirmed cases and deaths in China grew rapidly in January but started to decline in late February [3]. As of October 16, 2020, China reported 90,899 cases including 4739 deaths [1].

Despite scientific efforts, much about COVID-19 still remains uncertain, such as its origin and mutation [4]. Thus, given its high levels of risks, individuals are encouraged to take protective actions [5,6]. The extant research has explicated how individuals’ engagement in protective behavior against COVID-19 varied depending on their knowledge, fear, risk perception, morality, and internet use [7-12].

However, empirical evidence in China is still scarce. The authoritarian political system in China reduced resistance to the government’s executive orders such as locking down cities and placing citizens under quarantine [13], which controlled the spread of the pandemic [3]. In addition, the state ownership of media in China enables the government to provide large-scale health education and campaigns consistently, which might have facilitated engagement in protective actions. Thus, investigations on what factors affected Chinese individuals’ engagement in protective actions against COVID-19 may provide additional knowledge on the potential influence of a unique sociocultural environment on health behavior.

However, to the best of our knowledge, only one study was conducted on how Chinese individuals performed protective actions against COVID-19 [12]. Furthermore, that study is a one-time cross-sectional investigation at the early stage of the outbreak [12]. As it remains unknown when the pandemic might end, it is critical to examine how factors related to taking preventive measures against COVID-19 might change across different stages of its outbreak. Therefore, this study employs a repeated cross-sectional approach to address this limitation. Specifically, built on the extended parallel process model (EPPM) [13], this study aims to test the theory by examining how individuals’ risk perception of COVID-19 and their efficacy appraisal in controlling COVID-19 might affect their engagement in protective actions. Moreover, we seek to add to the extant research by investigating the role that one’s reliance on different sources for information about COVID-19 plays in performing these protective actions. Although we built our study on EPPM, other theoretical work such as the protection motivation theory [14], the health belief model [15], and the risk perception attitude framework [16] also considered variables in EPPM and made similar predictions.

EPPM

EPPM contends that whether individuals engage in protective behaviors depends on their risk perception and efficacy appraisal [17]. Risk perception is usually conceptualized as the sum or average of perceived severity and perceived susceptibility [18]. Perceived severity refers to one’s perception of the adversity of consequences if individuals do not engage in recommended actions, whereas perceived susceptibility is conceptualized as the likelihood that one is subject to the given health threat [18-20]. Additionally, efficacy appraisal is conceptualized as the sum of response efficacy and self-efficacy [18]. Response efficacy refers to the extent to which individuals think recommended protective actions can manage the given threat effectively, whereas self-efficacy is conceptualized as individuals’ confidence in performing those recommended acts [17,18].

The original research on EPPM posits that whether risk perception may facilitate engagement in protective actions depends on the level of efficacy appraisal [17,21]. Specifically, risk perception can only motivate individuals to perform protective actions at high levels of efficacy appraisal, whereas this positive relationship is absent at low levels of efficacy appraisal [17,21]. However, subsequent work demonstrated that risk perception can drive protective actions without high efficacy appraisal [22] because the innate aversion to loss prompts individuals to avoid potential risks by taking preventive measures [23]. Therefore, higher levels of perceived severity and perceived susceptibility may be associated with heightened motivation to perform protective actions [22,24-28].

In addition, individuals reporting high levels of response efficacy are more driven to engage in behaviors that can minimize the threat [26,29,30] because this confidence is often correlated with enhanced levels of hope [31]. Moreover, individuals reporting high levels of self-efficacy are more likely to follow the recommended acts because they tend to think it is less challenging to perform those behaviors [32]. Taken together, we predict that response efficacy and self-efficacy in controlling COVID-19 should exhibit positive relationships with engagement in protective behaviors.

Information Sources About COVID-19

Individuals equipped with accurate health information are usually more motivated to engage in health behaviors [9]. However, the volume of rumors about COVID-19 makes individuals vulnerable to health misinformation [33]. One factor that could potentially affect the credibility of information is its source. We categorized information sources into expert versus inexpert sources. Expert sources are conceptualized as individuals with medical expertise and organizations with professional gatekeepers that can screen information before it is published. These expert sources include expert media, government administrations, expert health organizations, and medical experts. The gatekeeping theory contends that gatekeepers, or people screening the information in these organizations, can enhance the accuracy of information [34]. Additionally, the heuristic-systematic model suggests that the public is inclined to trust the information provided through these sources because of its authority and thereby more motivated to follow the recommendations that these sources offer [35]. By contrast, inexpert sources are those lacking expertise background or professional gatekeepers, namely celebrities, social media influencers, and social contacts that are not doctors. Therefore, individuals relying on expert versus inexpert sources for information about COVID-19 may demonstrate different patterns of protective behaviors. Given these differences, we predicted that individuals relying on expert sources for information about COVID-19 should be more driven to engage in protective actions whereas reliance on inexpert sources should be related to engagement in protective actions negatively. As mentioned earlier, a repeated cross-sectional investigation will be employed. Hence, an additional question is whether these relationships changed throughout the duration of this study. Three protective actions were assessed: staying at home, wearing a face mask, and washing hands.


Overview

A six-wave repeated cross-sectional survey was conducted between February 7 and April 23, 2020, in collaboration with a large company that provides sampling services in China. Every other week, an online survey was distributed to a convenience sample of residents in six major cities in China. These cities were Beijing, Shanghai, Guangzhou, and Shenzhen, which are the four largest cities in China, as well as Wuhan, where the first COVID-19 cases were discovered [2], and Hangzhou, another city among the cities with the most reported cases [36].

Our survey started on February 7, 2020. Although cases were first found in Wuhan in late December 2019, the Chinese government did not inform the public that COVID-19 could be transmitted between humans until January 20 [37]. On January 23, Wuhan was locked down [38], which started a series of executive orders on travel bans and wearing face mask [13]. We did not start our research until February 7 because January 24 was the Lunar New Year’s Eve, which started a weeklong holiday. Therefore, we could not start our study until early February.

The data collection of wave 1 lasted from February 7-14, 2020. The second wave started on February 20 because most businesses in China restarted by late February and early March [39]. Thus, we wanted to investigate how the resumption of business might have affected our proposed relationships. Given the time difference between these two waves, we decided to collect our data every other week.

The lift of the lockdown in Wuhan on April 7, 2020, signaled the progress of pandemic control [40]. We collected the last wave (April 16-23) of data after April 7 to examine whether the lift of Wuhan’s lockdown might have changed our participants’ responses.

Sample

Table 1 presents the characteristics of the final sample in each wave. We matched the education and age of our sample to the national population. The most recent national census available to the public shows that around 14% of Chinese people received an associate’s degree or higher [41]. We also used this census to calculate the proportion of age strata in our sample: aged 18-30 years (19%), 31-45 years (26%), and 46 years and older (55%). However, this quota of education and age did not always match our sample characteristics in all waves.

Across all waves, there was no significant difference in biological sex (χ25=5.56, P=.35) and city of residence (χ225=6.99, P>.99). However, our participants differed significantly between waves in their age (F5,901.48=5.75, P<.001; one assumption of one-way variance of analysis is the homogeneity of variances in the dependent variable; however, this assumption was violated when age was compared across waves, so Welch was used to compare differences between waves), education (χ25=27.49, P<.001), and income (χ25=44.88, P<.001).

Table 1. Sample characteristics across waves.
CharacteristicsWave 1 (n=321)Wave 2 (n=319)Wave 3 (n=315)Wave 4 (n=343)Wave 5 (n=329)Wave 6 (n=315)
Sex, n (%)
Male154 (48)164 (51.4)141 (44.8)157 (45.8)153 (46.5)163 (51.7)
Female167 (52)155 (48.6)174 (55.2)186 (54.2)176 (53.5)152 (48.3)
Age (years), n (%)
18-3055 (17.1)64 (20.1)63 (20)84 (24.5)74 (22.5)60 (19)
31-4597 (30.2)82 (25.7)87 (27.6)110 (32.1)86 (26.1)80 (25.4)
≥46169 (52.6)173 (54.2)165 (52.4)149 (43.4)169 (51.4)175 (55.6)
Education, n (%)
Middle school or lower38 (11.8)21 (6.6)24 (7.6)10 (2.9)23 (7)50 (15.9)
High school234 (72.9)252 (79)249 (79)265 (77.3)250 (76)222 (70.5)
Associate’s degree or higher49 (15.3)46 (14.4)42 (13.3)68 (19.8)56 (17)43 (13.7)
Household monthly income (US $)
≤500, n (%)15 (4.7)9 (2.8)14 (4.4)7 (2)14 (4.3)25 (7.9)
501-714.29, n (%)31 (9.7)29 (9.1)22 (7)25 (7.3)21 (6.4)36 (11.4)
714.3-1142.86, n (%)55 (17.1)41 (12.9)48 (15.2)55 (16)49 (14.9)75 (23.8)
1142.87-1785.71, n (%)81 (25.2)95 (29.8)87 (27.6)93 (27.1)93 (28.3)89 (28.3)
1785.72-5500, n (%)113 (35.2)126 (39.5)128 (40.6)138 (40.2)130 (39.5)79 (25.1)
5500.01-11,928.57, n (%)20 (6.2)9 (2.8)9 (2.9)16 (4.7)16 (4.9)8 (2.5)
≥11,928.58, n (%)6 (1.9)10 (0.31)7 (2.2)9 (2.6)6 (1.8)3 (1)
Mean (SD)4.03 (1.32)4.15 (1.23)4.1 (1.24)4.21 (1.19)4.14 (1.24)3.63 (1.31)
City of residence, n (%)
Beijing55 (17.1)53 (16.6)52 (16.5)59 (17.2)52 (15.8)51 (16.2)
Shanghai54 (16.8)53 (16.6)51 (16.2)58 (16.9)52 (15.8)52 (16.5)
Guangzhou53 (16.5)53 (16.6)54 (17.1)71 (20.7)54 (16.4)50 (15.9)
Shenzhen53 (16.5)51 (16)53 (16.8)51 (14.9)64 (19.5)53 (16.8)
Wuhan52 (16.2)53 (16.6)52 (16.5)50 (14.6)53 (16.1)53 (16.8)
Hangzhou54 (16.8)56 (17.6)53 (16.6)54 (15.7)54 (16.4)56 (17.8)

Measures

Table 2 presents the reliability and descriptive statistics of independent and dependent variables in this study. Reliance on expert sources was measured by asking participants to indicate the extent to which their major source of information about COVID-19 was government health departments, government administrations, official media, medical institutes, medical experts, family and friends who are doctors, or the World Health Organization and other health organizations outside China (1=strongly disagree, 7=strongly agree). Reliance on inexpert sources was assessed by the same question except that the sources were replaced with celebrities, social media influencers, family and friends who are not doctors, and other social contacts who are not doctors. The reliability of these two variables at all waves reached .7 or above, except for reliance on inexpert sources, which was .66 at wave 4.

Gore and Bracken’s [42] 7-point Likert scale (1=strongly disagree, 7=strongly agree) was adapted to measure perceived severity, perceived susceptibility, response efficacy, and self-efficacy in controlling COVID-19. Specifically, perceived severity was measured with three questions (“COVID-19 is a very serious disease/will pose a severe threat to my health/will pose a severe threat to others’ safety”), and perceived susceptibility was measured with two items (“My chance to get COVID-19 is high” and “I can get COVID-19 from others”). Response efficacy was assessed with two items (“modern medical knowledge can control COVID-19” and “COVID-19 can be cured as long as one follows doctors’ recommendations”), and self-efficacy was assessed with three items (“I can follow the recommended acts to protect myself from COVID-19,” “I have no difficulty in performing those protective behaviors that the government recommended,” “I can master how to perform recommended actions”). The reliability of these three variables reached .7 or above across all waves except for self-efficacy, which was .69 at wave 2.

Table 2. Cronbach alpha, means, and SDs of major variables.
VariablesWave 1Wave 2Wave 3Wave 4Wave 5Wave 6
αMean (SD)αMean (SD)αMean (SD)αMean (SD)αMean (SD)αMean (SD)
Expert sources.785.41 (0.85).795.61 (0.79).735.61 (0.68).775.64 (0.74).785.60 (0.71).785.76 (0.73)
Inexpert sources.704.00 (1.04).753.95 (1.04).753.84 (1.03).663.93 (0.92).803.87 (1.04).783.98 (1.08)
Perceived severity.806.09 (1.03).836.26 (0.99).786.10 (1.00).816.23 (0.99).726.08 (0.91).816.24 (0.98)
Perceived susceptibility.724.24 (1.56).734.30 (1.63).793.92 (1.59).704.05 (1.50).744.06 (1.42).834.26 (1.59)
Response efficacy.705.42 (1.18).805.28 (1.33).725.39 (1.17).775.35 (1.27).705.32 (1.14).725.57 (1.13)
Self-efficacy.775.89 (0.95).695.86 (0.90).715.83 (0.87).705.95 (0.82).735.84 (0.81).785.90 (0.91)
Staying at homeN/Aa4.10 (1.00)N/A4.15 (0.99)N/A3.99 (0.96)N/A3.76 (1.08)N/A3.60 (1.09)N/A3.57 (1.11)
Wearing a face maskN/A4.75 (0.89)N/A4.78 (0.81)N/A4.79 (0.78)N/A4.81 (0.74)N/A4.80 (0.70)N/A4.70 (0.86)
Washing handsN/A4.72 (0.81)N/A4.80 (0.57)N/A4.75 (0.64)N/A4.70 (0.75)N/A4.74 (0.60)N/A4.68 (0.67)

aN/A: not applicable.

Personal engagement in protective measures was assessed through three 5-point Likert questions. Participants were asked how often they went out during the past 7 days (1=never, 2=once or twice, 3=three or four times, 4=five or six times, 5=seven times or more). We reverse coded participants’ response to this question, so the large number indicates staying at home more often. We also asked participants how often they wore a face mask and washed their hands during the past 7 days (1=never, 2=rarely, 3=sometimes, 4=often, 5=all the time). Again, larger numbers indicate higher frequency of wearing a face mask and washing hands.

Control variables were biological sex, age, education (recoded as 1=middle school or lower, 2=high school, 3=associate’s degree or higher), household monthly income, city of residence, self-rated health condition (1=very unhealthy, 5=very healthy), and knowledge. Knowledge was measured with 17 questions on the transmission of COVID-19, its medication, vulnerable population, and prevention methods. Participants received one point whenever they made a correct option. This made the maximum score 42 points.

Data Analysis

We employed the Kruskal-Wallis H test to examine if there was any difference between engagement in the three protective behaviors and if the level of engagement in these behaviors differed across time. In addition, we conducted repeated ordinal regression through SPSS 25 (IBM Corp) to test our predictions. At each wave, the dependent variables were entered into the model separately, along with control variables and independent variables. This analysis was repeated six times. Log odds ratios (ORs) and ORs along with their 95% CIs were reported to indicate the relationship between two variables.

The ordinal regression results are shown in the tables in the next section. Given the volume of these findings, results were presented separately with different sets of independent variables. Yet, ordinal regression was conducted with all independent variables listed in the tables.


Engagement in Protective Behaviors

Table 3 presents results of the comparisons between engagement in three protective behaviors. Significant differences were found in staying at home across all waves (χ25=110.01, P<.001). However, no significant differences were found in wearing a face mask (χ25=8.07, P=.15) and washing hands (χ25=10.81, P=.06) across time. In addition, across all six waves, we found significant differences consistently in the level of engagement in all three behaviors (Table 3).

Table 3. Differences in engagement in three protective behaviors across time.
BehaviorsWave 1Wave 2Wave 3Wave 4Wave 5Wave 6Chi-square (df)P value
Staying at home, mean4.104.153.993.763.603.57110.01 (5)<.001
Wearing a face mask, mean4.754.784.794.814.804.708.07 (5).15
Washing hands, mean4.724.804.754.704.744.6810.81 (5).06
Chi-square (df)205.69 (2)225.46 (2)252.35 (2)331.43 (2)367.66 (2)344.63 (2)N/AaN/A
P value<.001<.001<.001<.001<.001<.001N/AN/A

aN/A: not applicable.

The Effects of Perceived Severity and Perceived Susceptibility

Table 4 presents how perceived severity and perceived susceptibility predicted engagement in the three protective behaviors across time. Perceived severity of COVID-19 predicted staying at home positively at waves 2 and 6 (Table 4). Individuals perceiving COVID-19 as more severe were more likely to wear a face mask at waves 1 and 5 (Table 4). The effect of perceived severity on washing hands was significant at waves 2, 4, 5, and 6 (Table 4). Conversely, perceived susceptibility to COVID-19 only predicted staying at home at waves 1 and 3, and both relationships were negative (Table 4). The effects of perceived susceptibility on wearing a face mask and washing hands were not significant.

Table 4. The effects of perceived severity and perceived susceptibility on engagement in protective behaviors across time.
Time and protective behaviorsPerceived severityPerceived susceptibility
Log ORa (95% CI)OR (95% CI)Log OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)
Wave 1
Staying at home–0.18 (–0.42 to 0.06)0.84 (0.66 to 1.06)–0.22 (–0.37 to –0.07)**0.80 (0.69 to 0.93)
Wearing a face mask0.46 (0.05 to 0.87)*1.59 (1.06 to 2.39)–0.07 (–0.38 to 0.24)0.93 (0.69 to 1.27)
Washing hands0.31 (–0.04 to 0.66)1.36 (0.96 to 1.93)0.01 (–0.24 to 0.26)1.01 (0.79 to 1.30)
Wave 2
Staying at home0.26 (0.02 to 0.51)*1.30 (1.02 to 1.66)–0.06 (–0.20 to 0.09)0.95 (0.82 to 1.09)
Wearing a face mask0.34 (–0.09 to 0.77)1.41 (0.92 to 2.17)–0.29 (–0.63 to 0.04)0.75 (0.53 to 1.04)
Washing hands0.53 (0.20 to 0.85)**1.69 (1.22 to 2.35)–0.07 (–0.31 to 0.17)0.93 (0.73 to 1.19)
Wave 3
Staying at home–0.12 (–0.36 to 0.11)0.89 (0.70 to 1.12)–0.21 (–0.35 to –0.06)**0.81 (0.70 to 0.94)
Wearing a face mask0.05 (–0.36 to 0.47)1.06 (0.70 to 1.60)–0.15 (–0.45 to 0.14)0.86 (0.64 to 1.16)
Washing hands–0.06 (–0.40 to 0.27)0.94 (0.67 to 1.31)–0.16 (–0.37 to 0.05)0.85 (0.69 to 1.05)
Wave 4
Staying at home–0.16 (–0.38 to 0.06)0.85 (0.68 to 1.06)–0.06 (–0.20 to 0.09)0.94 (0.82 to 1.09)
Wearing a face mask0.04 (–0.37 to 0.45)1.04 (0.69 to 1.57)–0.02 (–0.34 to 0.31)0.99 (0.72 to 1.36)
Washing hands0.37 (0.10 to 0.64)**1.45 (1.10 to 1.90)–0.15 (–0.38 to 0.07)0.86 (0.69 to 1.07)
Wave 5
Staying at home0.11 (–0.14 to 0.36)1.12 (0.87 to 1.43)0.01 (–0.15 to 0.16)1.01 (0.86 to 1.18)
Wearing a face mask0.55 (0.12 to 0.98)*1.73 (1.13 to 2.66)0.12 (–0.19 to 0.43]1.13 (0.83 to 1.54)
Washing hands0.41 (0.07 to 0.74)*1.50 (1.07 to 2.10)–0.08 (–0.31 to 0.15)0.92 (0.73 to 1.16)
Wave 6
Staying at home0.28 (0.04 to 0.51)*1.32 (1.04 to 1.66)0.002 (–0.14 to 0.14)1.00 (0.87 to 1.15)
Wearing a face mask0.26 (–0.05 to 0.57)1.30 (0.95 to 1.77)–0.06 (–0.31 to 0.18)0.94 (0.74 to 1.20)
Washing hands0.31 (0.03 to 0.59)*1.36 (1.03 to 1.81)–0.09 (–0.40 to 0.23)0.92 (0.67 to 1.25)

aOR: odds ratio.

*P<.05.

**P<.01.

The Effects of Response Efficacy and Self-Efficacy

Table 5 shows how response efficacy and self-efficacy affected engagement in protective actions across time. At wave 1, response efficacy predicted staying at home and washing hands positively (Table 5). After wave 1, its effect on protective behaviors became weak. Individuals who reported higher levels of response efficacy were more likely to stay at home at wave 2 and wash hands at wave 4 (Table 5). Response efficacy was not significantly associated with wearing a face mask at any time. Self-efficacy did not predict any protective behavior at any time.

Table 5. The effects of response efficacy and self-efficacy on engagement in protective behaviors across time.
Time and protective behaviorsResponse efficacySelf-efficacy
Log ORa (95% CI)OR (95% CI)Log OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)
Wave 1
Staying at home0.30 (0.08 to 0.52)**1.35 (1.08 to 1.68)–0.04 (–0.33 to 0.26)0.97 (0.72 to 1.30)
Wearing a face mask–0.01 (–0.42 to 0.41)0.93 (0.69 to 1.27)0.24 (–0.27 to 0.74)1.27 (0.77 to 2.10)
Washing hands0.36 (0.04 to 0.68)*1.43 (1.04 to 1.97)–0.10 (–0.53 to 0.34)0.91 (0.59 to 1.40)
Wave 2
Staying at home0.31 (0.11 to 0.50)**1.36 (1.11 to 1.66)–0.04 (–0.34 to 0.27)0.96 (0.71 to 1.31)
Wearing a face mask0.05 (–0.36 to 0.46)1.05 (0.70 to 1.59)0.24 (–0.33 to 0.81)1.27 (0.73 to 2.24)
Washing hands0.01 (–0.30 to 0.32)1.01 (0.74 to 1.38)0.37 (–0.10 to 0.83)1.45 (0.91 to 2.30)
Wave 3
Staying at home0.08 (–0.13 to 0.28)1.08 (0.88 to 1.32)–0.15 (–0.44 to 0.14)0.86 (0.64 to 1.15)
Wearing a face mask–0.07 (–0.48 to 0.35)0.94 (0.62 to 1.42)–0.21 (–0.76 to 0.34)0.81 (0.47 to 1.40)
Washing hands–0.14 (–0.43 to 0.15)0.87 (0.65 to 1.17)0.25 (–0.15 to 0.65)1.28 (0.86 to 1.91)
Wave 4
Staying at home–0.09 (–0.27 to 0.08)0.91 (0.77 to 1.09)0.02 (–0.28 to 0.31)1.02 (0.76 to 1.37)
Wearing a face mask0.25 (–0.09 to 0.58)1.28 (0.92 to 1.79)0.03 (–0.52 to 0.58)1.04 (0.60 to 1.79)
Washing hands0.26 (0.01 to 0.51)*1.30 (1.01 to 1.66)–0.07 (–0.47 to 0.33)0.94 (0.63 to 1.39)
Wave 5
Staying at home0.18 (–0.04 to 0.39)1.19 (0.96 to 1.48)–0.17 (–0.47 to 0.13)0.84 (0.63 to 1.14)
Wearing a face mask0.15 (–0.27 to 0.58)1.17 (0.76 to 1.78)–0.15 (–0.70 to 0.40)0.86 (0.50 to 1.49)
Washing hands0.02 (–0.29 to 0.33)1.02 (0.75 to 1.39)0.28 (–0.13 to 0.69)1.33 (0.88 to 2.00)
Wave 6
Staying at home0.03 (–0.20 to 0.25)1.03 (0.82 to 1.28)0.05 (–0.26 to 0.36)1.05 (0.77 to 1.43)
Wearing a face mask–0.13 (–0.52 to 0.27)0.88 (0.59 to 1.31)0.15 (–0.32 to 0.62)1.16 (0.73 to 1.86)
Washing hands–0.09 (–0.40 to 0.23)0.92 (0.67 to 1.25)0.19 (–0.19 to 0.58)1.21 (0.83 to 1.78)

aOR: odds ratio.

*P<.05.

**P<.01.

The Effects of Reliance on Expert Versus Inexpert Sources

Table 6 demonstrates how individuals’ reliance on expert versus inexpert sources for information about COVID-19 might affect their engagement in the three protective actions across time. Reliance on expert sources did not predict engagement in any protective behaviors at wave 1, and only predicted wearing a face mask at wave 2 (Table 6). Starting from wave 3, the facilitating effect of expert sources became more prominent. Specifically, reliance on expert sources predicted staying at home positively at waves 3 and 4 (Table 6). In addition to wave 2, individuals relying on expert sources for information about COVID-19 were more likely to wear a face mask at waves 4, 5, and 6 (Table 6). The relationship between reliance on expert sources and washing hands was significant at waves 3, 4, 5, and 6 (Table 6).

The effect of reliance on inexpert sources on protective behaviors was more limited. Reliance on inexpert sources exhibited a negative effect on staying at home at wave 2 (Table 6). Individuals relying on inexpert sources were less likely to wear a face mask at wave 2 and wash hands at wave 5 (Table 6).

Table 6. The effects of reliance on expert versus inexpert sources on engagement in protective behaviors across time.
Time and protective behaviorsExpert sourcesInexpert sources
Log ORa (95% CI)OR (95% CI)Log OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)
Wave 1
Staying at home0.06 (–0.25 to 0.37)1.06 (0.78 to 1.45)0.04 (–0.18 to 0.25)1.04 (0.84 to 1.28)
Wearing a face mask–0.06 (–0.71 to 0.60)0.94 (0.49 to 1.81)–0.22 (–0.66 to 0.22)0.80 (0.52 to 1.24)
Washing hands0.33 (–0.16 to 0.82)1.39 (0.85 to 2.28)–0.34 (–0.72 to 0.05)0.71 (0.49 to 1.05)
Wave 2
Staying at home0.34 (–0.01 to 0.69)1.40 (0.99 to 1.99)–0.25 (–0.49 to –0.02)*0.78 (0.62 to 0.99)
Wearing a face mask0.68 (0.01 to 1.34)*1.97 (1.01 to 3.83)–0.50 (–0.97 to –0.03)*0.61 (0.38 to 0.97)
Washing hands0.39 (–0.13 to 0.92)1.48 (0.87 to 2.51)–0.27 (–0.65 to 0.12)0.77 (0.52 to 1.12)
Wave 3
Staying at home0.50 (0.11 to 0.89)*1.64 (1.11 to 2.42)–0.20 (–0.43 to 0.03)0.83 (0.65 to 1.03)
Wearing a face mask0.64 (–0.12 to 1.40)1.89 (0.89 to 4.05)–0.06 (–0.48 to 0.36)0.94 (0.62 to 1.43)
Washing hands0.76 (0.21 to 1.30)**2.13 (1.23 to 3.68)–0.15 (–0.48 to 0.17)0.86 (0.62 to 1.19)
Wave 4
Staying at home0.49 (0.17 to 0.82)**1.64 (1.18 to 2.27)–0.10 (–0.34 to 0.14)0.91 (0.71 to 1.15)
Wearing a face mask0.82 (0.20 to 1.44)*2.26 (1.22 to 4.22)–0.34 (–0.89 to 0.20)0.71 (0.41 to 1.23)
Washing hands0.96 (0.50 to 1.42)***2.61 (1.65 to 4.12)–0.04 (–0.39 to 0.31)0.96 (0.68 to 1.36)
Wave 5
Staying at home0.01 (–0.34 to 0.35)1.01 (0.71 to 1.42)–0.08 (–0.30 to 0.14)0.92 (0.74 to 1.15)
Wearing a face mask0.64 (0.09 to 1.20)*1.90 (1.09 to 3.30)–0.35 (–0.80 to 0.11)0.71 (0.45 to 1.11)
Washing hands0.59 (0.13 to 1.05)*1.80 (1.14 to 2.85)–0.37 (–0.71 to –0.03)*0.69 (0.49 to 0.97)
Wave 6
Staying at home0.08 (–0.30 to 0.46)1.08 (0.74 to 1.58)–0.22 (–0.44 to 0.003)0.80 (0.64 to 1.00)
Wearing a face mask0.64 (0.09 to 1.19)*1.90 (1.10 to 3.28)–0.19 (–0.54 to 0.16)0.83 (0.58 to 1.17)
Washing hands0.74 (0.27 to 1.22)**2.10 (1.31 to 3.39)–0.05 (–0.34 to 0.24)0.95 (0.71 to 1.28)

aOR: odds ratio.

*P<.05.

**P<.01.

***P<.001.

The Effects of Control Variables

Knowledge did not predict staying at home at any time. Individuals equipped with more knowledge were more likely to wear a face mask at wave 3 (OR 1.14, 95% CI 1.05-1.25; P<.01). The relationship between knowledge and washing hands was only significant and positive at wave 3 (OR 1.09, 95% CI 1.02-1.17; P<.05) and wave 6 (OR 1.08, 95% CI 1.01-1.15; P<.05).

The self-rated health condition predicted wearing a face mask (OR 2.21, 95% CI 1.27-3.85; P<.01) and washing hands positively at wave 4 (OR 1.63, 95% CI 1.10-2.41; P<.05). At wave 6, the relationship between self-rated health condition and staying at home was positive (OR 1.43, 95% CI 1.02-2.00; P<.05).

Income predicted staying at home negatively at wave 3 (OR 0.77, 95% CI 0.64-0.93; P<.01), wave 4 (OR 0.81, 95% CI 0.67-0.97; P<.05), and wave 6 (OR 0.83, 95% CI 0.69-1.00; P<.01). Individuals with a greater household monthly income were more likely to wear a face mask at wave 1 (OR 1.67, 95% CI 1.19-2.35; P<.01), wave 2 (OR 1.49, 95% CI 1.02-2.17; P<.05), wave 4 (OR 1.47, 95% CI 1.00-2.16; P<.05), and wave 6 (OR 1.43, 95% CI 1.08-1.89; P<.05). The relationship between income and washing hands was positive at wave 1 (OR 1.37, 95% CI 1.04-1.79; P<.05) and wave 4 (OR 1.37, 95% CI 1.06-1.77; P<.05).

Compared to women, men washed hands less often at wave 4 (OR 0.38, 95% CI 0.20-0.72; P<.01), wave 5 (OR 0.43, 95% CI 0.23-0.80; P<.01), and wave 6 (OR 0.50, 95% CI 0.28-0.92; P<.05). Age predicted washing hands positively at wave 1 (OR 1.04, 95% CI 1.01-1.08; P<.05), wave 4 (OR 1.06, 95% CI 1.02-1.09; P<.01), and wave 6 (OR 1.04, 95% CI 1.00-1.07; P<.05). At wave 4, participants with a high school degree washed hands less often than those with an associate’s degree or above (OR 0.36, 95% CI 0.15-0.89; P<.05).

When it comes to city differences, residents in Wuhan, where COVID-19 cases were first discovered, were used as the reference group. No significant difference was found in wearing a face mask and washing hands across all waves, except that residents in Beijing reported to wear a face mask more often than those in Wuhan (OR 9.69, 95% CI 1.09-86.38; P<.05). However, residents in Wuhan stayed at home more often than those in the other cities at most times, as Tables 7 and 8 shows.

Table 7. City differences in staying at home waves 1, 2, and 3 (Wuhan was used as the reference group).
CityWave 1Wave 2Wave 3
Log ORa (95% CI)OR (95% CI)Log OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)Log OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)
Beijing–0.62 (–1.38 to 0.13)0.54 (0.25 to 1.14)–1.76 (–2.57 to –0.95)***0.17 (0.08 to 0.39)–0.84 (–1.60 to –0.07)*0.43 (0.20 to 0.93)
Shanghai–0.99 (–1.75 to –0.22)*0.37 (0.17 to 0.80)–1.74 (–2.56 to –0.92)***0.18 (0.08 to 0.40)–1.32 (–2.10 to –0.54)**0.27 (0.12 to 0.58)
Guangzhou–0.87 (–1.62 to –0.11)*0.42 (0.20 to 0.89)–1.75 (–2.58 to –0.92)***0.17 (0.08 to 0.40)–0.72 (–1.49 to 0.04)0.49 (0.23 to 1.04)
Shenzhen–01.15 (–1.93 to –0.36)**0.32 (0.15 to 0.70)–1.58 (–2.41 to –0.75)***0.21 (0.09 to 0.47)–0.81 (–1.58 to –0.04)*0.44 (0.21 to 0.96)
Hangzhou–1.01 (–1.77 to –0.25)**0.37 (0.17 to 0.78)–1.33 (–2.18 to 0.47)**0.27 (0.11 to 0.62)–1.18 (–1.95 to –0.42)**0.31 (0.14 to 0.66)

aOR: odds ratio.

*P<.05.

**P<.01.

***P<.001.

Table 8. City differences in staying at home waves 4, 5, and 6 (Wuhan was used as the reference group).
CityWave 4Wave 5Wave 6
Log ORa (95% CI)OR (95% CI)Log OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)Log OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)
Beijing–1.26 (–2.02 to –0.51)**0.28 (0.13 to 0.60)–0.14 (–0.90 to 0.61)0.87 (0.41 to 1.84)–0.49 (–1.26 to 0.27)0.61 (0.28 to 1.31)
Shanghai–1.35 (–2.11 to –0.58)**0.26 (0.12 to 0.56)–0.63 (–1.40 to 0.14)0.53 (0.25 to 1.15)–0.99 (–1.78 to –0.21)*0.37 (0.17 to 0.81)
Guangzhou–1.35 (–2.07 to –0.64)***0.26 (0.13 to 0.53)–1.28 (–2.04 to –0.52)**0.28 (0.13 to 0.59)–0.52 (–1.32 to 0.28)0.59 (0.27 to 1.32)
Shenzhen–1.25 (–2.02 to –0.48)**0.29 (0.13 to 0.62)–0.73 (–1.44 to –0.02)*0.48 (0.24 to 0.99)–0.44 (–1.23 to 0.35)0.65 (0.29 to 1.42)
Hangzhou–1.75 (–2.52 to –0.98)***0.17 (0.08 to 0.38)–1.02 (–1.75 to –0.30)**0.36 (0.17 to 0.74)–0.55 (–1.31 to 0.21)0.58 (0.27 to 1.23)

aOR: odds ratio.

*P<.05.

**P<.01.

***P<.001.

Summary

Despite inconsistencies, some patterns still emerged. First, reliance on expert sources encouraged protective behaviors, but this effect did not emerge until wave 3 and was stronger on wearing a face mask and washing hands. Second, the discouraging effect of reliance on inexpert sources was limited to wave 2 except that it predicted washing hands negatively at wave 5. In addition, perceived severity exhibited a stronger effect on protective behaviors than perceived susceptibility. Furthermore, self-efficacy was not associated with engaging in protective behaviors, whereas the effect of response efficacy was limited to waves 1 and 2. Among all control variables, the effect of knowledge was limited, whereas the city of residence exhibited a stronger effect on staying at home.


Principal Findings

The COVID-19 pandemic triggered research on what factors affected individuals’ engagement in protective behaviors [7-12]. This study is built upon EPPM, a theoretical framework that explains how risk perception and efficacy appraisal might affect individuals’ engagement in protective behaviors [13]. In addition, given the volume of misinformation about preventive measures against COVID-19 [33], we extended EPPM and the extant research on protective actions against COVID-19 by recognizing the value of accurate information and considering Chinese individuals’ reliance on expert versus inexpert information sources. Further, differences across time and between three target behaviors were also revealed. The patterns of our findings previously summarized provide important implications on health education and suggest the intertwined relationship between one’s health behavior and the sociocultural system where these individuals reside.

First, we found that perceived severity could encourage protective behaviors, but their effects were not consistent and different depending on the specific behavior. Taken as a whole, perceived severity predicted washing hands positively at waves 2, 4, 5, and 6, more consistently than wearing a face mask (waves 1 and 5) and staying at home (waves 2 and 6). The inconsistency might be related to the executive orders that the Chinese government issued, which forced individuals to wear a face mask in public and placed them in quarantine [13]. Therefore, in this study, wearing a face mask and staying at home were not entirely autonomous decisions but more because of compliance with the executive orders. However, washing hands was not required, and it was impossible to ensure that everyone washed their hands as recommended. Thus, how often individuals washed their hands was likely derived from their evaluation of the risk.

Surprisingly, perceived susceptibility predicted staying at home negatively at waves 1 and 3. The post hoc analysis found that at both waves the common predictor of perceived susceptibility was self-rated health condition (wave 1: OR 0.72, 95% CI 0.56-0.92; P<.05; wave 3: OR 0.65, 95% CI 0.50-0.86; P<.01), and older participants reported a worse health condition (wave 1: OR 0.97, 95% CI 0.95-0.99; P<.01; wave 3: OR 0.95, 95% CI 0.93-0.97; P<.001). Therefore, among older participants, there might be a gap between risk perception and behavior. Although they realized that they could be subject to COVID-19, they still went out. This suggests that health education for seniors should focus on bridging the perception-behavior gap.

Overall, the effect of perceived susceptibility on protective behaviors was minimal. However, the impact of perceived susceptibility should not be dismissed. For example, protection motivation theory contends that human behavior is a function of the perceived severity of the threat, perceived susceptibility to the threat, and response efficacy, and no behavior is performed if any of these predictors are zero [14]. Although more empirical evidence is needed to understand whether health education in China during the pandemic lacks information on susceptibility, this result suggested that subsequent education should highlight the chance that certain populations are vulnerable to the pandemic.

In addition to risk perceptions, our results showed that response efficacy only predicted staying at home at waves 1 and 2, and washing hands at wave 1. Hence, at the early stage of the outbreak, individuals engaged in preventive measures because perhaps they believed these actions were effective to protect them against the given threat. This suggests that practitioners may want to adjust the emphasis of health education as time passes. Specifically, elevating response efficacy of the target audience may be important at the early stage of the outbreak.

By contrast, self-efficacy did not predict any protective behavior at any time. One possible reason is that our measure of self-efficacy addressed overall confidence in performing preventive measures instead of specific preventive actions. However, there might be differences in the level of difficulty in performing these three protective behaviors. Thus, our measure might not have assessed this subtle difference.

It is important to note that EPPM research tends to test the aggregate effects of perceived severity and perceived susceptibility as well as response efficacy and self-efficacy on protective behaviors [18,21,22,42,43]. However, we demonstrated the separate effects of these variables, and we found their distinct effects. This suggests that perceived severity versus perceived susceptibility (response efficacy vs self-efficacy) may be essentially different, which needs further study.

In addition to testing EPPM, our results demonstrated how reliance on expert versus inexpert sources might affect Chinese individuals’ engagement in protective actions. Our findings reveal that the positive effect of expert sources did not emerge until wave 3 when most businesses restarted [39]. The post hoc analysis found that, controlling for knowledge, self-rated health condition, and demographic variables, reliance on expert sources at wave 1 was significantly lower than all other waves (wave 2: OR 2.26, 95% CI 1.72-2.98; P<.001; wave 3: OR 1.56, 95% CI 1.19-2.04; P<.01; wave 4: OR 1.75, 95% CI 1.34-2.29; P<.001; wave 5: OR 1.54, 95% CI 1.17-2.02; P<.01; wave 6: OR 1.57, 95% CI 1.20-2.06; P<.01). One explanation is that it took time for the Chinese public to develop trust in these expert sources and follow the messages that these sources delivered. Expert sources in China, such as official media and health departments, are under strict control by the Chinese government, which was blamed for their failure to provide timely responses to COVID-19 during its early outbreak. This might have affected Chinese individuals’ trust in these expert sources given their close connection with the government. However, the aggressive actions that the government took controlled the spread of the pandemic and made the number of cases start to decline in late February 2020 [3,13]. Therefore, at wave 3, which started in early March, Chinese individuals might have gained more trust in these expert sources, making them more willing to comply with the recommendations that these sources offered. This suggests that individuals’ trust in information sources may exhibit a critical impact on their health behavior. Furthermore, this finding suggests that the conventional approach to persuading the public to engage in protective behaviors during the pandemic, which centers on knowledge provision, may not be effective. A more important mission might be to help the public develop trust in the community of public health practitioners including those working for the government. Therefore, a perspective of public relations is needed in future research and practices on health education.

In contrast, reliance on inexpert sources did not affect protective behaviors most of the time, except that these sources discouraged preventive measures at wave 2. This shows that our participants might have realized the risks of inexpert sources in information provision, so they did not follow this information. Although these findings are promising, information literacy should still be a focus of future health education and campaigns, especially those vulnerable to health misinformation, such as seniors and less educated individuals.

Additionally, the significant effect of reliance on inexpert sources was limited to wave 2. One possible explanation is that the public interest changed as time passed. In January and February 2020, the public may have been concerned about how to control and treat COVID-19. However, the restart of businesses might have signaled that the pandemic was under control. By then, individuals may have been more concerned about economic recession and recovery. Hence, after wave 2, the focus of the information exchanged between inexpert sources might have changed, which made reliance on these sources not significantly related to taking preventive measures.

Finally, the effects of several control variables warrant discussion. The impact of knowledge on protective behaviors was limited, and residents in Wuhan stayed at home more than participants in other cities at most times. These two findings can be explained by the influence of executive orders. The lockdown of Wuhan lasted more than 2 months, so naturally, participants from Wuhan stayed at home more. Additionally, the limited influence of knowledge suggests that Chinese individuals’ engagement in protective behaviors might not be a result of their autonomous decisions but compliance with executive orders. Although this approach to behavior change controlled the spread of COVID-19 in China [3,13], the duration of its effect is questionable, which future research needs to investigate.

Limitations and Future Research

These findings must be interpreted with several caveats. First, the cross-sectional nature of this study makes it impossible to build causal relationships between variables. Second, our study uses self-reported data. This method relies on participants’ memory and can be subject to social desirability.

In addition, as previously explained, Chinese individuals performed these protective actions partly because of their compliance with strict law enforcement and executive orders issued by China’s government. This might explain why our participants’ responses to questions measuring their engagement in protective behaviors were skewed. Furthermore, this might affect the validity of responses that our participants provided. Hence, social desirability must be considered when results are interpreted.

Although we matched the age and the education level of our sample to the national population in China, the generalizability of our sample may still be a limitation. Moreover, the proportions of education and age did not match the national population at all waves. The significant differences in education, income, and age between waves might have introduced additional variances and affected the validity of our results.

This study was conducted in China during the COVID-19 pandemic. This particular timing and geographic location might limit the generalizability of our results. Cross-cultural comparisons and longitudinal observations can be valuable directions for future research.

Our measures of self-efficacy and knowledge could also affect the validity of our findings. As mentioned earlier, the measure of self-efficacy did not specify the preventive behavior. Moreover, we self-created our scale of knowledge based on relevant information from the media. Established measures based on a manual provided by health departments would be more valid.

It is important to note that our definition of risk perception was limited to cognitive appraisal, which may dismiss the effect of affective responses. Future inquiries are needed to understand how cognitive and affective appraisals of risks may affect individuals’ engagement in protective behaviors during the pandemic.

Finally, as argued earlier, whether Chinese individuals engaged in protective behavior might partly be a result of strict executive orders. Thus, Chinese individuals’ attitude toward the political system may play a part in their engagement in protective behaviors against COVID-19. This implication may also apply in other countries such as the United States, where pandemic control has been politicalized [44,45]. Therefore, future research may need to examine how variables such as political interest and political orientation may affect one’s health behavior.

Conclusion

This study provides empirical evidence on what affected Chinese individuals’ engagement in protective behaviors against COVID-19 between February and April 2020. Given the authoritarian political system in the media, Chinese individuals’ engagement in protective behavior might not be an entirely autonomous decision but a result of compliance with executive orders. Our findings demonstrate that expert sources did not encourage protective behaviors until the early stage passed, suggesting that it might take time to develop trust in expert sources. Therefore, the effect of health education may depend on information as well as the relationship between practitioners and the public. This suggests that a perspective of public relations should be considered in future research. In addition, perceived severity could motivate some protective measures, but its effect differed depending on the specific behavior. Furthermore, the facilitating effect of perceived severity lasted throughout the duration of the pandemic but that of response efficacy was limited to the early stage. Hence, practitioners may want to adjust the emphasis of health campaigns depending on the stage of the pandemic.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Department of Education in Guangdong Province under grant 2019SFKC06.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

  1. COVID-19 Dashboard by the Center for Systems Science and Engineering (CSSE) at Johns Hopkins University (JHU). Johns Hopkins University.   URL: https://gisanddata.maps.arcgis.com/apps/opsdashboard/index.html#/bda7594740fd40299423467b48e9ecf6 [accessed 2020-07-31]
  2. WHO timeline - COVID-19. World Health Organization.   URL: https://www.who.int/news-room/detail/27-04-2020-who- timeline---covid-19 [accessed 2020-07-21]
  3. Kupferschmidt K, Cohen J. China’s aggressive measures have slowed the coronavirus. They may not work in other countries. Science. 2020 Mar 02.   URL: https:/​/www.​sciencemag.org/​news/​2020/​03/​china-s-aggressive-measures-have- slowed-coronavirus-they-may-not-work-other-countries [accessed 2020-07-21]
  4. Callaway E, Ledford H, Mallapaty S. Six months of coronavirus: the mysteries scientists are still racing to solve. Nature. 2020 Jul 03.   URL: https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-020-01989-z [accessed 2020-07-21]
  5. Community-based health care, including outreach and campaigns, in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. World Health Organization.   URL: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/WHO-2019-nCoV-Comm_health_care-2020.1 [accessed 2020-07-21]
  6. Collecting open government approaches to COVID-19. Open Government Partnership.   URL: https://www.opengovpartnership.org/collecting-open-government-approaches-to-covid-19/ [accessed 2020-07-21]
  7. Clements J. Knowledge and behaviors toward COVID-19 among US residents during the early days of the pandemic: cross-sectional online questionnaire. JMIR Public Health Surveill 2020 May 08;6(2):e19161 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Harper C, Satchell L, Fido D, Latzman R. Functional fear predicts public health compliance in the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Ment Health Addict 2020 Apr 27:1-14 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Li S, Feng B, Liao W, Pan W. Internet use, risk awareness, and demographic characteristics associated with engagement in preventive behaviors and testing: cross-sectional survey on COVID-19 in the United States. J Med Internet Res 2020 Jun 16;22(6):e19782 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Taghrir M, Borazjani R, Shiraly R. COVID-19 and Iranian medical students; a survey on their related-knowledge, preventive behaviors and risk perception. Arch Iran Med 2020 Apr 01;23(4):249-254. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Wise T, Zbozinek T, Michelini G, Hagan CC, Mobbs D. Changes in risk perception and protective behavior during the first week of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. PsyArXiv Preprint posted online March 19, 2020. [CrossRef]
  12. Zhong B, Luo W, Li H, Zhang Q, Liu X, Li W, et al. Knowledge, attitudes, and practices towards COVID-19 among Chinese residents during the rapid rise period of the COVID-19 outbreak: a quick online cross-sectional survey. Int J Biol Sci 2020;16(10):1745-1752 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Lhatoo Y. Never mind China, look to the US for the next big coronavirus crisis. South China Morning Post. 2020 Mar 07.   URL: https:/​/www.​scmp.com/​news/​hong-kong/​health-environment/​article/​3074087/​never-mind-china-look-us- next-big-coronavirus [accessed 2020-07-21]
  14. Rogers R. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. J Psychol 1975 Sep;91(1):93-114. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Janz N, Becker M. The Health Belief Model: a decade later. Health Educ Q 1984;11(1):1-47. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Rimal R, Real K. Perceived risk and efficacy beliefs as motivators of change: use of the risk perception attitude (RPA) framework to understand health behaviors. Human Comm Res 2003 Jul 01;29(3):370-399. [CrossRef]
  17. Witte K. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: the extended parallel process model. Commun Monogr 1992 Dec;59(4):329-349. [CrossRef]
  18. Witte K. Fear as motivator, fear as inhibitor: using the extended parallel process model to explain fear appeal successes and failures. In: Andersen PA, Guerrero LK, editors. Handbook of Communication and Emotion: Research, Theory, Applications, and Contexts. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press; 1996:423-450.
  19. van der Pligt J. Risk perception and self-protective behavior. Eur Psychol 1996 Jan;1(1):34-43. [CrossRef]
  20. Rimal RN. Perceived risk and self-efficacy as motivators: understanding individuals' long-term use of health information. J Commun 2001 Dec;51(4):633-654. [CrossRef]
  21. Wong N, Cappella J. Antismoking threat and efficacy appeals: effects on smoking cessation intentions for smokers with low and high readiness to quit. J Appl Commun Res 2009;37(1):1-20 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav 2000 Oct;27(5):591-615. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Barkley-Levenson E, Van Leijenhorst L, Galván A. Behavioral and neural correlates of loss aversion and risk avoidance in adolescents and adults. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2013 Jan;3:72-83 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Hadlaczky G, Hökby S, Mkrtchian A, Wasserman D, Balazs J, Machín N, et al. Decision-making in suicidal behavior: the protective role of loss aversion. Front Psychiatry 2018;9:116. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. de Hoog N, Stroebe W, de Wit JBF. The impact of vulnerability to and severity of a health risk on processing and acceptance of fear-arousing communications: a meta-analysis. Rev Gen Psychol 2007 Sep 01;11(3):258-285. [CrossRef]
  26. Floyd DL, Prentice-Dunn S, Rogers RW. A meta-analysis of research on protection motivation theory. J Appl Soc Psychol 2000 Feb;30(2):407-429. [CrossRef]
  27. Tannenbaum M, Hepler J, Zimmerman R, Saul L, Jacobs S, Wilson K, et al. Appealing to fear: a meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories. Psychol Bull 2015 Nov;141(6):1178-1204 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Sheeran P, Harris P, Epton T. Does heightening risk appraisals change people's intentions and behavior? A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Psychol Bull 2014 Mar;140(2):511-543. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Lam S. Predicting intention to save water: theory of planned behavior, response efficacy, vulnerability, and perceived efficiency of alternative solutions. J Appl Social Pyschol 2006 Nov;36(11):2803-2824. [CrossRef]
  30. Lewis I, Watson B, White K. Response efficacy: the key to minimizing rejection and maximizing acceptance of emotion-based anti-speeding messages. Accid Anal Prev 2010 Mar;42(2):459-467. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Nabi R, Prestin A. Unrealistic hope and unnecessary fear: exploring how sensationalistic news stories influence health behavior motivation. Health Commun 2016 Sep;31(9):1115-1126. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Bandura A. Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ Behav 2004 Apr;31(2):143-164. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Cuan-Baltazar J, Muñoz-Perez MJ, Robledo-Vega C, Pérez-Zepeda MF, Soto-Vega E. Misinformation of COVID-19 on the internet: infodemiology study. JMIR Public Health Surveill 2020 Apr 09;6(2):e18444 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. Shoemaker P, Vos T. Gatekeeping Theory. New York, NY: Routledge; 2009.
  35. Cummings L. The "trust" heuristic: arguments from authority in public health. Health Commun 2014;29(10):1043-1056. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Diao M, Zhang S, Chen D, Hu W. The novel coronavirus (COVID-19) infection in Hangzhou: an experience to share. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol 2020 Jul;41(7):874-875 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. Kuo L. China confirms human-to-human transmission of coronavirus. The Guardian. 2020 Jan 21.   URL: https:/​/www.​theguardian.com/​world/​2020/​jan/​20/​coronavirus-spreads-to-beijing-as-china-confirms-new-cases [accessed 2020-07-21]
  38. Crossley G. Wuhan lockdown 'unprecedented', shows commitment to contain virus: WHO representative in China. Reuters. 2020 Jan 23.   URL: https://www.reuters.com/article/us-china-health-who-idUSKBN1ZM1G9 [accessed 2020-07-21]
  39. Singh S, Leng C, Sun Y. China businesses to restart at larger scale by late Feb, early March: Ministry of Transport. Reuters. 2020 Feb 22.   URL: https:/​/www.​reuters.com/​article/​us-china-health-business/​china-businesses-to-restart-at-larger- scale-by-late-feb-early-march-ministry-of-transport-idUSKCN20G09N [accessed 2020-07-21]
  40. Jeffery A. Coronavirus: photos of Wuhan after 11-week lockdown. CNBC. 2020 Apr 08.   URL: https:/​/www.​cnbc.com/​2020/​04/​08/​wuhan-lifts-travel-restrictions-after-11-week-lockdown-see-photos.​html [accessed 2020-07-21]
  41. The 2018 Yearbook of Statistics in China. National Bureau of Statistics in China.   URL: http://www.stats.gov.cn/tjsj/ndsj/2018/indexch.htm [accessed 2020-07-21]
  42. Gore T, Bracken C. Testing the theoretical design of a health risk message: reexamining the major tenets of the extended parallel process model. Health Educ Behav 2005 Feb;32(1):27-41. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Chen L, Yang X. Using EPPM to evaluate the effectiveness of fear appeal messages across different media outlets to increase the intention of breast self-examination among Chinese women. Health Commun 2019 Oct;34(11):1369-1376. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Painter M, Qiu T. Political beliefs affect compliance with COVID-19 social distancing orders. SSRN J 2020:1. [CrossRef]
  45. Calvillo D, Ross B, Garcia R, Smelter T, Rutchick A. Political ideology predicts perceptions of the threat of COVID-19 (and susceptibility to fake news about it). Soc Psychological Pers Sci 2020 Jul 22;11(8):1119-1128. [CrossRef]


EPPM: extended parallel process model
OR: odds ratio


Edited by A Kushniruk; submitted 05.08.20; peer-reviewed by D Chrimes, S Kim; comments to author 11.10.20; revised version received 29.10.20; accepted 24.11.20; published 12.01.21

Copyright

©Jian Raymond Rui, Keqing Yang, Juan Chen. Originally published in JMIR Human Factors (http://humanfactors.jmir.org), 12.01.2021.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Human Factors, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on http://humanfactors.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.