Original Paper
Abstract
Background: Spending time in natural environments is beneficial for human health, but many older adults have limited or no access to natural environments. Virtual reality technology may be a means to facilitate nature experiences, and so, there is a need for knowledge on how to design virtual restorative natural environments for older adults.
Objective: The aim of this study was to identify, implement, and test older adults’ preferences and ideas regarding virtual natural environments.
Methods: A total of 14 older adults (mean age 75, SD 5.9 years) participated in an iterative process to design such an environment. We used think-aloud protocols and qualitative content analysis and established questionnaires that targeted usability, affective aspects, and side effects. These data guided the design decisions for incremental implementations of a prototype.
Results: The participants’ preferences included trueness to reality in terms of rendition and behavior; traces of human activity and natural processes that trigger the imagination and provide believability; the ability to roam, explore, and interact with the environment; and a familiar, relatable environment that evokes memories. The iterative design process resulted in a prototype featuring many of the participants’ ideas and preferences, including a seated locomotion technique, animals, a boat ride, the discovery of a boat wreck, and apple picking. The questionnaire results indicated high perceived usability, interest, and enjoyment; low pressure and tension; moderate value and usefulness; and negligible side effects.
Conclusions: We suggested 3 principles for virtual natural environments for older adults: realness, interactivity, and relatedness. Virtual natural environments should also provide a diversity of content and activities to accommodate the heterogeneity in older adults’ preferences. These results can contribute to a framework for designing virtual natural environments for older adults. However, these findings need to be tested and potentially revised in future studies.
doi:10.2196/40932
Keywords
Introduction
Background
Currently, there is much evidence that spending time in natural environments can be beneficial for cognitive function, mental health, and well-being [
- ]. Unfortunately, many older adults, who may be in particular need of such health benefits, have limited or no access to natural environments. For example, older adults living in residential care facilities may have a diminished ability to go outside because of limitations in functioning that are associated with old age. A few studies have suggested that replacements for real nature experiences such as indoor gardens can have positive effects [ , ]. A study found significant improvements in sleep, agitation, and cognition among 23 institutionalized patients with dementia who were allowed to cultivate and care for easy-to-grow edible plants indoors for 28 days [ ]. Virtual reality (VR) has also been suggested as a way for older adults who cannot go outside to spend time in virtual natural environments (VNEs) [ , ].A systematic review of indoor nature interventions (indoor gardens, plants, photographs, films, and 1 nonimmersive VR forest presented on a single large screen) for older adults in residential care settings found mixed results [
]. There was not sufficient evidence to recommend such interventions over other interventions or activities. In addition, interventions that involved photos, films, or nonimmersive VR were less effective than interactions with real forms of nature such as indoor gardening. However, immersive VR technology such as head-mounted displays (HMDs), where the viewer is completely surrounded by the virtual environment, has a higher potential to provide a sense of presence (ie, the perceptual illusion of being there) than nonimmersive media such as photos and single-screen displays [ - ]. Thus, although nonimmersive media would be unlikely to override the residents’ sense of indoor presence, a VNE experienced through immersive VR technology may provide residents with the perceptual illusion of being outdoors in nature.A related study [
] compared different types of mediated nature experiences by depicting tropical coral reef scenes. The results showed that real-time 3D computer graphics via an HMD elicited a greater sense of presence, nature connectedness, and positive affect than filmed footage via a single screen. Another study comparing different delivery methods of psychotherapeutic interventions found reports of greater positive affect, satisfaction, and perceived credibility by participants exposed via an HMD than by those exposed via a single-screen display or using mental imagery [ ]. Immersive VR has also been shown to induce physiological reactions in test participants similar to those induced by a corresponding real situation [ ]. With this in mind, one can easily imagine the potential of VR to provide immersive experiences of simulated natural environments to care facility residents who have limited or no access to real natural environments. Recently, a few studies were carried out with older adults experiencing VNEs through immersive technology (HMDs). These studies reported some positive results, such as displayed enjoyment and relaxation [ ] and positive responses, and that VNEs soothed and evoked memories [ ] and improved positive affect and nature connectedness [ ].VNE studies often cite theories of restoration in natural environments [
], such as attention restoration theory [ ], stress reduction theory [ ], and the biophilia hypothesis [ ]. Restoration theories can provide guidance for the design of restorative environments, for example, the 4 components by Kaplan [ ]: being away, extent, soft fascination, and compatibility. Nukarinen et al [ ] presented a framework connecting restoration theories and the measurement of health outcomes in VNE studies.However, VR presents both limitations and possibilities that are different from those of a real natural environment. For example, VR cannot yet mimic the complexity, dynamic behavior, and immense detail of a real natural environment. On the other hand, it provides more or less complete control over the form and function of the virtual environment. Hence, a designer of VNEs is faced with choices that are not applicable in a real natural environment. Moreover, a user’s perception of an artifact is colored by the context in which it resides [
]. As VR provides a perceptual but not cognitive illusion [ ]—that is, it feels real, but the user is aware that it is not—artifacts in VR are perceived differently from their corresponding objects in actual reality. As an example, a viewer may be impressed by how real a moss-covered rock looks in VR but may think nothing of a similar rock in the real world. Therefore, the knowledge of real restorative natural environments may not be applicable to virtual ones. To our knowledge, there are no frameworks, models, or guidelines for designing VNEs that are based on knowledge generated through a bottom-up approach in a VR context.In general, older adults are seldom involved in participatory design processes; in particular, they are not involved in the development of new technologies such as VNEs [
]. As a result, older adults are rarely given a voice in the development of technological solutions aimed at them, which in turn may lead to the rejection or nonadoption of the technological solution in question [ ].Research Questions
In this study, we aimed to identify, implement, and test preferences and ideas for VNEs by involving older adults in an iterative design process of a prototype. Thus, we explored the following research questions: (1) What preferences and ideas do older adults have for a VNE? and (2) How can we realize them?
We present our design process, the outcomes of 3 iterations, and some suggestions for what ought to be considered by designers when designing VNEs for older adults. We hope that our description of the explorative design process, along with interpretations and reflections on older adults’ preferences for VNEs, are a valuable contribution for designers and researchers alike and that our study may serve to indicate future directions in the development of VNEs for older adults.
Methods
Overview
This study was conducted as a user-centered design process of a VNE to elicit the needs and preferences of users, challenge assumptions, and explore design ideas [
]. Users representing the target group were invited to iteratively test and provide feedback on the VNE prototype in a laboratory environment. Data were collected through a think-aloud protocol and questionnaires. Qualitative data were analyzed using an inductive qualitative content analysis method inspired by Graneheim and Lundman [ ]. A total of 3 iterations were performed. The results of the data analyses of the previous iterations served to inform the design choices in the subsequent implementation phases. By iteratively developing the prototype and testing and analyzing the participants’ reactions ( ), we were able to generate and test design concepts in small increments and, thus, build the VNE from the bottom up based on user input throughout the process. After completing the final iteration, we continued the analysis with a focus on the underlying threads of meaning running through all the data [ ].Ethics Approval
Ethics approval for this study was obtained from the Regional Ethical Review Board, Lund, Sweden (2017/118).
Test Setup
The tests were performed in a laboratory environment equipped with recording equipment for video and audio. The VNE prototype was developed and tested on a Windows PC with a GeForce GTX 1080 graphics card and an HTC Vive VR headset. We used the HTC Vive’s room-scale tracking capabilities and dedicated a play area of approximately 3 × 4 m within the laboratory.
Each test session comprised the following six steps (steps 1-3 were only included in the participants’ first sessions): (1) the participant was welcomed to the laboratory and given an explanation of the background of the study; (2) the participant was invited to undergo a short demonstration session to familiarize themselves with using the VR system; (3) the participant was asked to fill out a background questionnaire covering their previous experiences with VR and natural environments; (4) the participant underwent a concurrent think-aloud test session (described in the Think-Aloud Protocol section) while using the prototype (this session was video recorded); (5) the participant was asked to fill out questionnaires measuring usability, affective aspects, and side effects; and (6) the participant underwent a retrospective think-aloud session (described in the Think-Aloud Protocol section) while watching the recorded material from the concurrent think-aloud session (this session was also video recorded).
Participants
A total of 14 participants (n=9, 64% women and n=5, 36% men) were recruited via retiree organizations in southern Sweden. Participants’ ages ranged from 69 to 90 years. The mean age was 75 (SD 5.9) years, and the median age was 73 years. The inclusion criteria were being aged >65 years, being able to speak and understand Swedish, having adequate eyesight to watch television, and being able to transport themselves to the laboratory. Exclusion criteria were any propensity for dizziness or motion sickness, dementia, reduced cognitive function, and problems with balance. The sampling was purposeful as the participants were intentionally selected to elucidate the VNE prototype from the perspective of an older person, and the sampling was convenient in the sense that the participants themselves chose to sign up for the study rather than being randomly selected [
].In total, 57% (8/14) of the participants took part in the first iteration (
). All of these participants (8/8, 100%) also took part in the second iteration along with 3 new individuals, resulting in 79% (11/14) of the total participants testing the second version of the VNE prototype. In the third iteration, 93% (13/14) of the total participants took part, whereof 23% (3/13) were new recruits and 23% (3/13) had participated in the second iteration.Iteration | P1 | P2 | P3 | P4 | P5 | P6 | P7 | P8 | P9 | P10 | P11 | P12 | P13 | P14 |
1 | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ||||||
2 | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | |||
3 | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ |
Think-Aloud Protocol
During the test sessions, we used an adapted concurrent and retrospective think-aloud protocol [
]. Before each concurrent think-aloud session, the participant was asked to speak freely about their experience while using the VNE and share their ideas and suggestions for how they thought it should be changed or further developed. When necessary, the first author would use directive probing techniques [ ] in an attempt to cover aspects of the VNE that the participant had not yet addressed, for example, by asking, “What is your perception of the water?” To allow the participant to go into greater depth in their reasoning, the first author would sometimes attempt nondirective probing techniques [ ]. The retrospective think-aloud session was conducted in the same manner, only instead of using the VNE, the participant watched a video recording of the concurrent think-aloud session with the first author ( ).Questionnaires
Although this was primarily a qualitative study, standardized questionnaires (
) were included in each iteration as a complement to gain an understanding of how our implementations affected the usability, affective aspects, and side effects of the prototype. Usability was measured using the System Usability Scale (SUS) [ ], which is a widely used method for measuring the perceived usability of a system. It consists of 10 statements rated by the user on a 5-point Likert scale about a system’s characteristics, including complexity, ease of use, consistency, and learnability, for example, “I found the various functions in this system were well integrated.” Bangor et al [ ] created the following adjective rating scale to interpret SUS scores: Worst imaginable, Awful, Poor, OK, Good, Excellent, and Best imaginable. Affective aspects were measured using the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) [ ], which is a multidimensional measure that can be used for assessing a person’s experience of an activity. The IMI can be customized to only include the dimensions relevant to a particular use. We chose to include the following dimensions: interest and enjoyment, value and usefulness, and felt pressure and tension. We chose to exclude the following dimensions: perceived competence, effort, and perceived choice. Side effects were measured using the Virtual Reality Symptom Questionnaire [ ]. The questionnaire consists of 13 questions measuring physical symptoms that may be experienced when using a VR system, such as headache or nausea, rated by the user on a 7-point scale. In their first test sessions, the participants were also asked to fill out a background questionnaire regarding their past and present experiences of natural environments, their previous level of experience with VR, and their state of well-being at the moment.Analysis
After each iteration, the notes from the think-aloud sessions were analyzed by one of the authors (RL) using the recordings as a reference when necessary. Each idea, preference, opinion, suggestion, or other thought expressed by the participants was coded using an inductive content analysis method inspired by Graneheim and Lundman [
]. By searching for patterns within the codes, categories emerged that guided the decision-making when implementing new functions, content, and other changes in the VNE prototype.In the implementation stages of each iteration, authors RL and MW discussed the categories (hereinafter referred to as preferences) to reach an agreement on how they should be interpreted from a design perspective, that is, how the preferences could be realized as changes to the prototype. RL then implemented the agreed-upon changes. As the participants reacted to the new features in the subsequent tests, the preferences that had been realized within the prototype were to some extent validated by the participants themselves. They would sometimes build on and develop them through more specific requests, reflections, suggestions, or ideas. Thus, there was a progression and deepening of ideas over the course of the iterations. We attempted to reflect this in this paper by presenting the preferences in the order in which they were realized.
After completing the final iteration, we continued the analysis assuming a bird’s-eye view of the data. By reflecting on the patterns and interrelationships of the preferences, we arrived at a set of principles for VNEs for older adults. All the authors reached a consensus on the principles by means of repeated discussion.
The Initial Prototype
In the construction of the VNE prototype, we used computer-generated real-time 3D graphics via the Unreal Engine (version 4; Epic Games, Inc) game engine. Thus, it generated visuals, audio, and other potential sensory outputs in the moment based on user input and programmed behavior. The virtual environment was a compound of authored and sampled (from the real world) materials such as 3D models, images, and sound files. To leave room for the participants’ ideas, we designed the initial prototype to be a rather bare and simple starting point. As the subject of this study was the limited access to natural environments in one’s own region, the features included in the scene were of the type that one might expect to see in the geographical region in which the study was set. After putting on the HMD, the participants would find themselves next to a lake near a forest (
) in a scene comprising various 3D models of grass, moss, water, rocks, sand, dirt, trees, leaves, flowers, and natural debris such as old twigs and logs. Other than the water and grass, which were animated to simulate movement caused by the wind, all objects were static. The environment was lit like a sunny summer day, and the sounds of small birds could be heard. The participants entered the simulation standing up and were able to move around freely over the play area using their own bodies as they would in the real world. This freedom to move allowed the participants to turn around and obtain a complete 360° view of the scene, inspect details such as flowers on the ground by moving closer to them, and look behind objects such as rocks and trees.Results
Overview
The average concurrent and retrospective think-aloud sessions lasted 29 and 23 minutes, respectively. Analyses of the sessions yielded several categories reflecting various topics and containing both the participants’ opinions of the current iteration of the VNE along with ideas, preferences, and suggestions for future versions. These were not necessarily exclusive to a particular iteration; however, we present them in an order that shows the progression of the design process. Hence, for each iteration, we present the ideas, preferences, and suggestions that inspired the implementations made in that particular iteration or that are linked to it in other ways. By doing so, we aimed to show how participation guided the design rather than being tokenistic.
Participants
The participants generally had very little experience with VR but much access to and experience of spending time in natural environments (
). All except 1 of the 14 participants (13/14, 93%) answered “yes” to the question “Do you have access to a garden in connection with your home?” Generally, the participants reported high perceived well-being at the moment. Some background questions were left out of this table.Question | Mean score (SD) | Median score |
How much experience do you have with using VRa? (1=none; 5=very much) | 1.1 (0.4) | 1 |
How much experience do you have of natural environments? (1=none; 5=very much) | 3.9 (0.9) | 4 |
How great are your possibilities to spend time in natural environments? (1=none; 5=very great) | 3.9 (1.1) | 4 |
How often do you spend time in natural environments? (1=never; 5=very often) | 3.2 (0.8) | 3 |
How do you feel? (1=very bad; 5=perfectly good) | 4.3 (0.8) | 4.5 |
aVR: virtual reality.
The First Iteration
During the first iteration, the participants tested the initial prototype, spoke their preferences and ideas during the think-aloud sessions, and filled out the questionnaires.
shows the results of the IMI ( A) and SUS ( B) questionnaires. The IMI scores indicated that most participants (7/8, 88%) found the experience to be highly interesting and enjoyable and somewhat useful and valuable to them and did not feel much pressure or tension. The median SUS score was 85, which is just below excellent on the adjective scale by Bangor et al [ ]. An analysis of the think-aloud data is presented in the following section.The Second Iteration
To begin the second iteration, we analyzed the think-aloud data from the first iteration.
presents the participants’ preferences along with suggestions and illustrative quotes from the participants. As can be seen, they preferred more movement, life, and change and the ability to roam and explore the environment but also sit and relax, and they emphasized the importance of authenticity and realism.Preferences | Description | Participants’ suggestions | Illustrative quotes |
Movement, life, and change | The environment was perceived as static and sterile and lacked life, change, and movement. | Add animals, changes in the light from cloud movements, and movement in the trees from the wind. |
|
Roaming and exploring | Participants wanted to move beyond the play area and roam and explore the environment. | —a |
|
Sitting, passive enjoyment, and relaxation | Participants wanted to sit and relax in the environment, enjoying the view. | Add the possibility to sit on a recliner, bench, blanket on the ground, or in a boat on the water and have a picnic. |
|
Authenticity and realism | In various ways, participants emphasized the importance of authenticity and realism in the VNEb. | There should be movement in the trees from the wind just as there is in the grass; otherwise, it is inconsistent. |
|
aNot available.
bVNE: virtual natural environment.
To continue with the next step in the second iteration, we proceeded to interpret the participants’ preferences and suggestions in terms of how we could realize them as changes to the prototype.
presents the implementations along with the preferences they addressed and the reactions of the participants. We added various animals, movement of the trees, the possibility to roam and explore the environment, and a jetty on the lake. As can be seen, we implemented 2 different ways to enable the participants to roam and explore the environment. Although teleportation is commonplace in many VR applications, the experience of teleporting is fundamentally different from walking as one does not perceive any gradual movement, and participants had specifically expressed a wish to take walks. Therefore, we realized the need for a technique closer to walking. An obvious solution was to simply implement forward propulsion in the direction of the forward vector of either the HMD or the handheld controller at the push of a button on the controller. However, many older people are wheelchair users or experience reduced postural stability and may lose their balance and fall because of vection. We speculated that there would be very few residents at care facilities who could manage immersive VR while standing up. We also considered that a seated locomotion technique might accommodate to some degree the participants’ preference for sitting, passive enjoyment, and relaxation.In light of this reasoning, we implemented a system in which a user could sit down while driving around in the virtual world. This was accomplished by fastening one of the handheld controllers to the back of a swivel chair, allowing the chair’s position and orientation to be tracked by the VR application (
). In the virtual environment, the chair was represented by a simple 3D model. The virtual chair’s position and orientation were updated in real time to correspond to those of the real chair. To control the throttle, the user could press a button on the other handheld controller while sitting in the swivel chair, which would result in them experiencing forward propulsion in the direction of the chair in the virtual environment. To steer, the user would simply turn the chair in the direction in which they wanted to go using their feet. Thus, the user would not experience circular vection while steering, something that is associated with motion sickness [ ].At the end of the second iteration, the participants tested the new prototype while thinking aloud, reacting to the new implementations and providing new preferences and suggestions. As before, they filled out the questionnaires.
presents some quotes that illustrate the participants’ reactions. shows the IMI ( A) and SUS ( B) scores. The IMI scores indicated that the participants found the experience of the new VNE to be highly enjoyable and interesting, that they did not feel too pressured or tense, and that the value and usefulness of the experience to them was neutral to high. The median SUS score was just above good on the adjective scale by Bangor et al [ ]. This indicated that, although the complexity of use increased with the implementation of teleportation and the swivel-chair vehicle, usability remained satisfactory.Implementations | Addressed preferences | Description | Reactions of the participants |
Animals | Movement, life, and change; authenticity and realism | Birds, fish, and a butterfly exhibiting natural-like behavior controlled by AIa scripts |
|
Movement of the tree branches from the wind | Movement, life, and change; authenticity and realism | Animation of the tree branches to resemble movement from the wind |
|
Teleportation | Roaming and exploring | Ability to instantly teleport oneself by aiming the handheld controller to an arbitrary point in the environment and pressing a button |
|
Swivel-chair vehicle | Roaming and exploring; sitting, passive enjoyment, and relaxation | Ability to drive in the environment while sitting in a swivel chair, press a controller button to instigate propulsion, and steer by turning oneself in the desired direction |
|
Jetty | Roaming and exploring; movement, life, and change | A jetty to accommodate exploring the lake and observing the fish |
|
aAI: artificial intelligence.
The Third Iteration
presents the participants’ preferences and suggestions that were expressed during the think-aloud sessions of the previous iteration (second iteration). Preferences that emerged included bodies of water; the ability to look at, study, or inspect things; a rich diversity of activities; and human presence or activity.
We proceeded to analyze how the participants’ new preferences could be realized and made additions and changes to the prototype accordingly.
presents these implementations, the preferences they were meant to address, and some of the participants’ reactions that were gathered during the subsequent think-aloud sessions that concluded the third iteration. As shown in , the implementations consisted of a small rowboat and a peninsula ( A), a sunken boat ( B), and an apple tree ( C and D).shows the IMI ( A) and SUS ( B) scores for the third iteration. The IMI scores indicated that the participants found the experience highly interesting and enjoyable and did not feel much pressure or tension and that their view of its value and usefulness to them was neutral to high. The median SUS score was just above excellent on the adjective scale by Bangor et al [ ], which again indicated that, despite the increased complexity of use that was introduced with the rowboat and the apple tree, the participants viewed the prototype as highly usable.
Preferences | Description | Participants’ suggestions | Illustrative quotes |
Water | Participants expressed a preference for bodies of water and for being in or on the water. | Add the possibility to ride in or drive a boat such as a rowboat or canoe. |
|
Interactivity | Participants had many diverse preferences for activities in the VNEa. | Add the ability to pick things such as flowers, mushrooms, apples, or shells; go fishing, canoeing, and mountain climbing; and read a book, grill, swim, fly a kite, and play with their grandchildren. |
|
Look at, study, or inspect things | Participants wanted to look at, study, or inspect interesting things in the environment. | Provide the possibility to look down into the water and see things such as clams, crabs, aquatic plants, or just the bottom; inspect birds closely; and look through binoculars. |
|
Human presence or activity | Participants thought that one should be able to see or hear humans, human activity, or traces thereof. | People who are visibly present in the distance, walking by, swimming, or working in a garden; occasional sounds from agriculture, forestry, or a car in the distance; an airplane in the sky; and items or structures that reveal human presence, such as a bench, a fireplace, an old bicycle, or an old boat |
|
aVNE: virtual natural environment.
Implementations | Addressed preferences | Description | Reactions of the participants |
Small rowboat | Water; look at, study, or inspect things; interactivity; human presence or activity | A small rowboat that a user could enter and drive on the water |
|
Sunken boat | Look at, study, or inspect things; human presence or activity | A sunken boat that could be discovered by driving past in the rowboat |
|
Peninsula | Realism and authenticity; roaming and exploring | Reshaping of the landscape to form a peninsula in the lake; adding more trees and flowers with the intention of making the environment look more authentic and natural and more interesting to explore |
|
Apple tree | Interactivity; human presence or activity | An apple tree from which users could pick apples and place them in a basket |
|
Final Analysis
Upon completion of the third iteration, we proceeded to analyze the latest round of think-aloud sessions.
presents both preferences that relate to the latest implementations and those that surfaced throughout the study that did not pertain to any particular iteration or implementation. Participants reflected that discovering traces of human activity triggered their imagination. Generally, participants preferred the environment to be familiar and relatable. Other ideas that surfaced recurringly throughout the study were the ability to eat or drink, that the sound should be more realistic and varied, and being able to touch and smell the environment and feel the wind.Preferences | Description | Participants’ suggestions | Illustrative quotes |
Discovering traces of human activity triggers the imagination. | Participants reflected that traces of human presence or activity made the environment more interesting and set their imaginations in motion. They wondered what events had taken place, which people had been there before, and what had happened to them. | Add more traces to discover a cairn, an old pot, ruins of an old house, traces of a garden, an old well, a root cellar, or an item that someone had lost or dropped. Make it so things change in between sessions—someone picked all the apples or hauled away the shipwreck. |
|
Familiarity and reminiscence | Participants preferred the environment to be familiar and to be able to identify with it and relate to memories, such as from their childhood. | Make it possible to carry out activities one did as a child. |
|
Eating or drinking and socializing | Participants expressed a desire to eat or drink something, potentially in the form of a social situation such as having coffee together with others present in the VNEa at the same time. | The possibility to have a picnic on a blanket on the ground, a wooden bench table, or a cafeteria |
|
Sound | Participants thought that sounds that realistically should be there were missing and that there should be more variation. | Sounds from the wind, rustling when walking over fallen leaves, and splashing of water when driving the boat; sounds that bring variation: a cuckoo or pigeon, a car in the distance, or people walking |
|
Other modalities | Participants wanted to experience the environment through additional senses. | To feel the breeze and the warmth of the sun; touching things, trees, rocks, or apples; smelling things, flowers, or coffee; having feet lowered into a bucket of water while being in the virtual water |
|
aVNE: virtual natural environment.
Observations of Usability
Although the SUS scores indicated high usability throughout the study, in our observations of the participants exploring the environment, we noticed that some had difficulties finding the correct buttons on the controller for teleporting and driving. Sometimes, they teleported by mistake by accidentally pushing the teleport button. The task of approaching the apple tree and ending up in a location convenient for picking apples seemed difficult at times. This was because the participants often came too close to the tree or even moved inside it. The participants often flinched when they came too close to the branches.
VR Symptoms
The Virtual Reality Symptom Questionnaire revealed very few symptoms, with means of <1 on a scale from 0 to 6 on every symptom measured in all 3 iterations. However, there were isolated medium-high scores reported: a score of 3 for dizziness in iteration 2 and 2 scores of 4 for blurred vision in iterations 1 and 2.
Principles of a Meaningful VNE
Overview
As a final step in the process, we took a bird’s-eye view of all the participants’ preferences to reveal recurring threads of meaning and condense the findings into applicable principles. By reflecting on the patterns and interrelationships of the preferences, 3 main principles emerged—realness, interactivity, and relatedness. These could be considered by designers of future VNEs. However, they need to be tested and potentially revised in future studies.
Realness
Realness refers to how complete the experience is in terms of presence, realism, and believability. A VNE should provide a sense of presence (ie, a sense of being in place). This can be accomplished by providing (approximate) real-world responses to actions [
]. Sideways head movements, for example, should allow the user to look behind objects, and touching objects should optimally provide haptic feedback:It would be very interesting to sense that feeling [touch the rock]...For me, it would be very positive. Because then I am absolutely in nature.
[P6]
A VNE should provide believability. By this we mean a sense of a complete world that does not end behind a backdrop—a world in which there are interesting things to discover, such as animal and plant life, traces of human activity, details that reveal natural processes, and distant sounds. We propose that these characteristics can contribute to a feeling of a complex living world that continues beyond the reach of one’s eye and stretches back into a historical past:
It’s a very static environment. I must say that I don’t feel like I’m out in nature for real, more like I’m standing on a stage with backdrops.
[P11]
It’s more alive, just that little butterfly alone contributes a lot, I must say.
[P1]
[H]ere has been a house, and there you can see an old apple tree...it triggers the imagination, that someone has lived here and how were they doing? How were they able to live here in the middle of nowhere? Such things can also contribute a little.
[P5]
It provides realism to it all. I mean, it’s certainly not unusual that there lies an old miserable-looking boat by the shoreline.
[P14]
The trees look too nice...there are none [branches] that has been broken off by the wind and such.
[P5]
A VNE should provide a sense of trueness to reality, such as accuracy in rendition and the combination and behavior of natural and human-made elements:
It should be flowers that exist in reality.
[P1]
I don’t expect to see an apple tree in a pine forest.
[P11]
You stop enjoying it, lose interest in it...when you go into the details and it is incorrect. Then it’s maybe a little lesser of an experience.
[P9]
Interactivity
In addition to basic VR sensory-motor contingencies—including moving one’s body, head, and eyes to change gaze direction and looking behind or under things—a VNE should preferably provide the possibility to engage in activities (eg, exploring the environment and picking mushrooms). Activities should be congruent with the users’ capabilities, interests, and identities. Care should be taken to also accommodate for passive activities (eg, to just sit and relax looking at things). As previously stated, user actions should elicit responses that are as close to reality as possible:
To be able to do different things, walk around, look down into the water properly, drive the boat, pick apples. They are positive elements in it compared to the first time, [which was] just a quiet and rather flat environment. Suddenly a lot of things happen, you can do a lot of things.
[P5]
It was a lot of fun to pick the apples and put them in the basket. It was an extra-experience...because you do something, and you see that it works. But, if you drop an apple, normally, “thud” it says, but these were very soft.
[P13]
Relatedness
A VNE should enable a user to identify with it and relate to memories (eg, the environment should be familiar, and the vegetation and animals should be of species that one can recognize):
[It should be an environment] that you recognize yourself in, that you can relate to.
[P3]
Where I used to walk when I was a child, for example, I think would be a very nice experience...Because then you get it related directly to yourself.
[P13]
I have some good friends who have a family farm by a small lake...I can imagine that if they saw this, it would evoke memories in them in their old age; but to evoke such memories in me, it should be by the sea.
[P10]
To me, when it comes to nature...you relate to the memories that you have from nature, and if it matches better, it feels like it’s more natural.
[P8]
Discussion
Principal Findings
This study was conducted to investigate older adults’ preferences and ideas regarding VNEs and how these can be realized. To our knowledge, this is the only study in which such preferences for VNEs were collected through an iterative participatory design process using an inductive approach. Our results suggest the preferences outlined in
.It should be noted that preferences for activities were very diverse among our participants. Overall, we propose that one should pay attention to multiple choices of activities and variations of features. Ideally, a VNE should be tailored to the individual.
The participants’ responses to the questionnaires and reactions to our implementations suggest feasible ways to realize some of the preferences for VNEs that emerged during this study (
).Participant preferences.
Preferences for realness
- Trueness to reality in the rendition and behavior of natural and human elements (eg, accuracy in prevalence and combination of species)
- Real-world responses to actions, such as accurate audial and visual feedback (eg, displacement of objects and sound of impact)
- Extended range of sensory modalities such as touch, smell, and temperature
Preferences for interactivity
- The possibility to roam and explore the environment and inspect interesting things such as natural and human-made elements
- The possibility to engage in activities such as picking mushrooms, taking a boat ride, or having a picnic
- The possibility to sit and just relax observing the environment
Preferences for relatedness
- A familiar, relatable environment in which one can recognize oneself
- Being able to recognize and identify species and natural features
- Being able to relate to memories
Feasible ways to realize participant preferences.
Realizations of realness
- The occurrence of animals (eg, birds, fish, and insects) and the movement of vegetation and other natural features (eg, from wind) make the environment appear more alive and real.
- Traces of human activity trigger the imagination, allowing users to picture people and past events in the environment. This contributes to their perception of the environment as a real place.
Realizations of interactivity
- The possibility to roam and explore the environment enhances the experience of a virtual natural environment. It can activate and provide a sense of increased freedom to users.
- Teleportation and forward propulsion with seated swivel chair–directed steering are feasible methods for roaming and exploring the environment. However, they can be perceived as unnatural in their own ways.
- Activities that involve some manipulation of the environment, such as picking apples, can enhance the experience, making users feel involved and active. However, unrealistic mechanics of activities may be unfavorable for the experience.
Some of our results showed that participants wanted to be active in the VNE. They wanted to explore, inspect, and interact with the environment (eg, pick the flowers, touch the rocks, and feel the water). At the same time, they wanted their actions to be true to reality. This is not so surprising as perception requires action [
]. For example, shifting one’s head sideways enables one to see behind things. According to Slater et al [ ], VR works by providing real-world responses to actions. That is how one acquires a sense of presence, of being in place in the virtual environment. If one performs an action and there is no response or the response is too far from reality, one will momentarily break presence. Thus, consistent with our findings, we propose that the possibility of interaction (that approximates real-world interaction) is important in VNEs. Although this proposal may seem obvious, many studies of VNEs use technologies that feature minimal or no interaction, such as 360° video [ ], which does not support sideways head movements. However, 360° video has other advantages, such as the ability to capture existing environments, which can make the participants’ preference for relatedness (ie, familiar environments and the possibility to relate to memories) more economical and feasible to realize. Orr et al [ ] used 360° video to provide VR experiences of local beaches to older adults with mild to moderate cognitive or memory impairment. They found that familiarity with the environments brought the participants enjoyment in identifying places and relating to memories. They also found indications that the VR provided was “sufficiently credible” and that participants were able to acquire a sense of presence. However, the presence of other (filmed) people in the 360° videos provoked in the participants a desire to interact, which, naturally, was impossible. A recent study of VNE use by patients with breast cancer advised that future studies should “focus on activities that encourage connection with nature (rather than simply exposure to nature)” [ ].The biophilia hypothesis argues that humans have a biologically based need for a sense of belonging to the natural world. This connectedness with nature is instrumental for human well-being according to the hypothesis [
]. One can speculate that our participants’ preference for being able to identify with and relate to the environment is partly an expression of nature connectedness, and interaction with the environment has the potential to promote connectedness and relatedness. Other studies have measured nature connectedness in VNEs. One study [ ] found higher nature connectedness in participants using an immersive VNE than in those watching a corresponding natural environment on a television screen. Another study [ ] suggested that nature connectedness mediated positive affect in older adults using an immersive VNE.On the basis of the participants’ preferences and reactions to our implementations, we propose that a VNE for older adults should be true to reality in rendition and behavior and that traces of human activity and natural processes can promote the perception of it as a real place. This is very reminiscent of what Slater et al [
] present as a contributing factor to plausibility (Psi), which is the illusion that the events happening in the virtual environment are actually taking place. In addition to trueness to reality in appearance and behavior, Psi relies on the virtual environment in some way acknowledging the user (eg, that a virtual human character plausibly responds to an action by the user). Although this study did not involve social interaction with virtual human characters, we found that our proposal is congruent with the presentation of Psi by Slater et al [ ].Limitations and Future Research
The focus of this study leaned heavily toward gathering the participants’ ideas, preferences, and suggestions and revealing unforeseen problems and considerations in the process of designing a VNE. This focus was a determining factor in the choice to include the think-aloud protocols in the study design. There is a risk that the extensive recurring think-aloud sessions generated bias within the participants, which could have affected the scoring of the questionnaires. It is not difficult to imagine that the participants may have adopted a positive stance as they had someone who was listening carefully to them and was genuinely interested in their opinions, as well as experiencing firsthand that their opinions had an impact on the design of an important innovation. As the participants generally had very little experience with VR (
), its novelty could have contributed to the high IMI scores. The findings of this study need to be tested and potentially revised in future studies.As is evident from the inclusion and exclusion criteria and the results of the background questionnaires, the participants in this study were relatively healthy and mobile and had access to natural environments. Thus, they differed in many ways from the proposed group that this study is aimed at, namely, residents of residential care facilities who have limited or no access to natural environments because of ill health. The lower scores the participants gave on the value and usefulness dimension of the IMI questionnaire compared with the other dimensions may be a testament to this fact—they simply may not have found the VNE very useful as they were able to visit real natural environments. This study was also carried out in a controlled laboratory environment and so did not consider the complexity of residential care facilities and the network of different stakeholders involved. Consequently, the next step would be to continue these research efforts with residents and staff at care facilities in the real world.
The HTC Vive has a setting to adjust the distance between the lenses to match a user’s interpupillary distance (IPD). In some cases, an inaccurate IPD setting may cause visual distortion and discomfort [
]. Ideally, our test protocol should have included measuring each participant’s IPD and configuring their headset accordingly. However, to simplify the test procedure, we opted to set the IPD to 63 mm, which is the average in adults [ ], and only adjusted the IPD setting for users if they reported discomfort or an unacceptable image quality. The participants’ reports of blurred vision may partly be explained by an incorrect IPD setting. Future study protocols should include measuring the participants’ IPD to improve the perceived image quality and reduce the risk of eye strain and discomfort.Conclusions
This paper described an iterative user-centered design process for a VNE for older adults. We presented the participants’ preferences and ideas, how these were realized in the ongoing development of a VNE prototype, and how new implementations were received by the participants. We proposed 3 principles for VNEs for older adults: realness, interactivity, and relatedness. We also provided suggestions that can be considered by designers and researchers of VNEs for older adults. These include trueness to reality in terms of rendition and behavior; traces of human activity and natural processes that trigger the imagination and provide believability; the ability to roam, explore, and interact with the environment; and a familiar, relatable environment that evokes memories. VNEs should provide a diversity of content and activities to accommodate the heterogeneity in older adults’ preferences. We argued that non–VR-related theories of restorative natural environments may not be applicable in a VR context. This study can contribute to the development of a framework for designing VNEs for older adults.
Acknowledgments
This study was funded by the European Union Horizon 2020 research and innovation funding program under grant agreement 666773. The authors would like to thank all the participants in this study for giving their time and effort to test and share their ideas, preferences, suggestions, and opinions.
Authors' Contributions
Study conception, study design, material preparation, and analysis were conducted by the first author (RL) and the last author (MW). Software development and data collection were conducted by RL. The first draft of the manuscript was written by RL. All authors were involved in the discussions of the analysis, commented on previous versions of the manuscript, and read and approved the final manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
RL and MW are planning to register a business to provide virtual reality services for older adult care.
Questionnaires used in this study.
DOCX File , 25 KBReferences
- Bratman G, Hamilton J, Daily G. The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2012 Feb;1249(1):118-136. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Gascon M, Triguero-Mas M, Martínez D, Dadvand P, Forns J, Plasència A, et al. Mental health benefits of long-term exposure to residential green and blue spaces: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2015 Apr 22;12(4):4354-4379 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hartig T, Mitchell R, de Vries S, Frumkin H. Nature and health. Annu Rev Public Health 2014 Mar 18;35(1):207-228. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Keniger L, Gaston K, Irvine K, Fuller R. What are the benefits of interacting with nature? Int J Environ Res Public Health 2013 Mar 06;10(3):913-935 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- McMahan EA, Estes D. The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. J Positive Psychol 2015 Jan 13;10(6):507-519. [CrossRef]
- Sandifer PA, Sutton-Grier AE, Ward BP. Exploring connections among nature, biodiversity, ecosystem services, and human health and well-being: opportunities to enhance health and biodiversity conservation. Ecosystem Serv 2015 Apr;12:1-15. [CrossRef]
- Yao Y, Chen K. Effects of horticulture therapy on nursing home older adults in southern Taiwan. Qual Life Res 2017 Apr 1;26(4):1007-1014. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Mcsweeney J, Rainham D, Johnson SA, Sherry SB, Singleton J. Indoor nature exposure (INE): a health-promotion framework. Health Promot Int 2015 Mar 24;30(1):126-139. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lee Y, Kim S. Effects of indoor gardening on sleep, agitation, and cognition in dementia patients--a pilot study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008 May;23(5):485-489. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- White MP, Yeo N, Vassiljev P, Lundstedt R, Wallergård M, Albin M, et al. A prescription for “nature” – the potential of using virtual nature in therapeutics. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2018 Nov;14:3001-3013. [CrossRef]
- Depledge MH, Stone RJ, Bird WJ. Can natural and virtual environments be used to promote improved human health and wellbeing? Environ Sci Technol 2011 Jun 01;45(11):4660-4665. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Yeo N, Elliott L, Bethel A, White M, Dean S, Garside R. Indoor nature interventions for health and wellbeing of older adults in residential settings: a systematic review. Gerontologist 2020 Apr 02;60(3):e184-e199. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Slater M. Immersion and the illusion of presence in virtual reality. Br J Psychol 2018 Aug 21;109(3):431-433. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Makransky G, Terkildsen TS, Mayer RE. Adding immersive virtual reality to a science lab simulation causes more presence but less learning. Learn Instruction 2019 Apr;60:225-236. [CrossRef]
- Shu Y, Huang Y, Chang S, Chen M. Do virtual reality head-mounted displays make a difference? A comparison of presence and self-efficacy between head-mounted displays and desktop computer-facilitated virtual environments. Virtual Reality 2018 Nov 26;23(4):437-446. [CrossRef]
- Yeo NL, White MP, Alcock I, Garside R, Dean SG, Smalley AJ, et al. What is the best way of delivering virtual nature for improving mood? An experimental comparison of high definition TV, 360° video, and computer generated virtual reality. J Environ Psychol 2020 Dec;72:101500 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Frewen P, Mistry D, Zhu J, Kielt T, Wekerle C, Lanius RA, et al. Proof of concept of an eclectic, integrative therapeutic approach to mental health and well-being through virtual reality technology. Front Psychol 2020 Jun 5;11:858 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Jönsson P, Wallergård M, Osterberg K, Hansen M, Johansson G, Karlson B. Cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity and habituation to a virtual reality version of the Trier Social Stress Test: a pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010 Oct;35(9):1397-1403. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Appel L, Kisonas E, Appel E, Klein J, Bartlett D, Rosenberg J, et al. Administering virtual reality therapy to manage behavioral and psychological symptoms in patients with dementia admitted to an acute care hospital: results of a pilot study. JMIR Form Res 2021 Feb 03;5(2):e22406 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Orr N, Yeo NL, Dean SG, White MP, Garside R. "It makes you feel that you are there": exploring the acceptability of virtual reality nature environments for people with memory loss. Geriatrics (Basel) 2021 Mar 12;6(1):27 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chan SH, Qiu L, Esposito G, Mai KP, Tam K, Cui J. Nature in virtual reality improves mood and reduces stress: evidence from young adults and senior citizens. Virtual Real 2021 Nov 26:1-16 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Frost S, Kannis-Dymand L, Schaffer V, Millear P, Allen A, Stallman H, et al. Virtual immersion in nature and psychological well-being: a systematic literature review. J Environmental Psychol 2022 Apr;80:101765. [CrossRef]
- Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J Environmental Psychol 1995 Sep;15(3):169-182. [CrossRef]
- Ulrich RS, Simons RF, Losito BD, Fiorito E, Miles MA, Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Environmental Psychol 1991 Sep;11(3):201-230. [CrossRef]
- Kellert S, Wilson E. The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington, DC: Island press; 1995.
- Nukarinen T, Rantala J, Korpela K, Browning MH, Istance HO, Surakka V, et al. Measures and modalities in restorative virtual natural environments: an integrative narrative review. Comput Human Behav 2022 Jan;126:107008. [CrossRef]
- Krippendorff K, Butter R. Semantics: meanings and contexts of artifacts. In: Product Experience. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Science; 2008.
- Östlund B, Fischer B, Marshall B. Using academic work places to involve older people in the design of digital applications. Presentation of a methodological framework to advance co-design in later life. In: Human Aspects of IT for the Aged Population. Technologies, Design and User Experience. Cham: Springer; 2020.
- Peine A, Rollwagen I, Neven L. The rise of the “innosumer”—rethinking older technology users. Technol Forecast Soc Change 2014 Feb;82:199-214. [CrossRef]
- Gould JD, Lewis C. Designing for usability: key principles and what designers think. Commun ACM 1985 Mar;28(3):300-311. [CrossRef]
- Graneheim UH, Lundman B. Qualitative content analysis in nursing research: concepts, procedures and measures to achieve trustworthiness. Nurse Educ Today 2004 Feb;24(2):105-112. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Koerber A, McMichael L. Qualitative sampling methods. J Business Technical Commun 2008 Oct 01;22(4):454-473. [CrossRef]
- Ericsson K, Simon H. Protocol Analysis, Revised Edition Verbal Reports as Data. London: MIT Press; 1993.
- Moerman G. Probing Behaviour in Open Interviews : A Field Experiment on the Effects of Probing Tactics on Quality and Content of the Received Information. Amsterdam: VU University; 2010.
- Brooke J. SUS: a retrospective. J Usability Stud 2013;8(2):29-40.
- Bangor A, Kortum P, Miller J. Determining what individual SUS scores mean: adding an adjective rating scale. J Usability Stud 2009;4(3):123.
- McAuley E, Duncan T, Tammen VV. Psychometric properties of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory in a competitive sport setting: a confirmatory factor analysis. Res Q Exerc Sport 1989 Mar;60(1):48-58. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ames SL, Wolffsohn JS, McBrien NA. The development of a symptom questionnaire for assessing virtual reality viewing using a head-mounted display. Optom Vis Sci 2005 Mar;82(3):168-176. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Nooij SA, Pretto P, Oberfeld D, Hecht H, Bülthoff HH. Vection is the main contributor to motion sickness induced by visual yaw rotation: implications for conflict and eye movement theories. PLoS One 2017 Apr 5;12(4):e0175305 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Slater M, Banakou D, Beacco A, Gallego J, Macia-Varela F, Oliva R. A separate reality: an update on place illusion and plausibility in virtual reality. Front Virtual Real 2022 Jun 27;3. [CrossRef]
- O'Regan JK, Noë A. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav Brain Sci 2001 Oct 18;24(5):939-73; discussion 973. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chin S, Cavadino A, Akroyd A, Tennant G, Dobson R, Gautier A, et al. An investigation of virtual reality nature experiences in patients with metastatic breast cancer: secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Cancer 2022 Jul 22;8(3):e38300 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kim J, An BH, Jeong W, Lee S. Estimation of interpupillary distance based on eye movements in virtual reality devices. IEEE Access 2021;9:155576-155583. [CrossRef]
- Dodgson N. Variation and extrema of human interpupillary distance. In: Proceedings Volume 5291, Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XI. 2004 Presented at: Volume 5291, Stereoscopic Displays and Virtual Reality Systems XI; Jan 18-24, 2004; San Jose California USA. [CrossRef]
Abbreviations
HMD: head-mounted display |
IMI: Intrinsic Motivation Inventory |
IPD: interpupillary distance |
Psi: plausibility |
SUS: System Usability Scale |
VNE: virtual natural environment |
VR: virtual reality |
Edited by A Kushniruk; submitted 09.07.22; peer-reviewed by P Lindner, R Zhong; comments to author 08.11.22; revised version received 24.02.23; accepted 26.02.23; published 07.04.23
Copyright©Rikard Lundstedt, Johanna Persson, Carita Håkansson, Susanne Frennert, Mattias Wallergård. Originally published in JMIR Human Factors (https://humanfactors.jmir.org), 07.04.2023.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Human Factors, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://humanfactors.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.